• Nie Znaleziono Wyników

Różnice indywidualne w uczeniu się języka angielskiego jako obcego w środowisku blended learning

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Różnice indywidualne w uczeniu się języka angielskiego jako obcego w środowisku blended learning"

Copied!
315
0
0

Pełen tekst

(1)

Edyta Olejarczuk

Investigating the role of individual

differences in learning English as a

foreign language in a blended learning

environment

Różnice indywidualne w uczeniu się

języka angielskiego jako obcego w

środowisku blended learning

Praca doktorska napisana na Wydziale Anglistyki Uniwersytetu im. Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu pod kierunkiem prof. zw. dr. hab. Mirosława Pawlaka

(2)

Poznań, dnia 1.12.2015r.

OŚWIADCZENIE

Ja, niżej podpisana Edyta Olejarczuk przedkładam rozprawę doktorską pt: Investigating the role of individual differences in learning English as a foreign language in a blended learning environment na Wydziale Anglistyki Uniwersytetu im. Adama Mickiewicza w Poznaniu i oświadczam, że napisałam ją samodzielnie. Oznacza to, że przy pisaniu pracy, poza niezbędnymi konsultacjami, nie korzystałam z pomocy innych osób, a w szczególno-ści nie zlecałam opracowania rozprawy lub jej częszczególno-ści innym osobom, ani nie odpisywałam tej rozprawy lub jej części od innych osób. Oświadczam również, że egzemplarz pracy dy-plomowej w formie wydruku komputerowego jest zgodny z egzemplarzem pracy dyplo-mowej w formie elektronicznej.

Jednocześnie przyjmuję do wiadomości, że przypisanie sobie, w pracy dyplomowej, autorstwa istotnego fragmentu lub innych elementów cudzego utworu lub ustalenia nauko-wego stanowi podstawę stwierdzenia nieważności postępowania w sprawie nadania tytułu zawodowego.

[TAK]* - wyrażam zgodę na udostępnianie mojej pracy w czytelni Archiwum UAM [TAK]* - wyrażam zgodę na udostępnianie mojej pracy w zakresie koniecznym do ochrony mojego prawa do autorstwa lub praw osób trzecich

*Należy wpisać TAK w przypadku wyrażenia zgody na udostępnianie pracy w czytelni Archiwum UAM, NIE w przypadku braku zgody. Niewypełnienie pola oznacza brak zgody na udostępnianie pracy.

(3)

Table of contents

TABLE OF CONTENTS ... III LIST OF TABLES ... VII LIST OF FIGURES ... X

INTRODUCTION ... 1

CHAPTER 1 : INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION ... 4

1.1.DEFINITIONS AND TAXONOMIES OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES (IDS) ... 5

1.2.OVERVIEW OF SELECTED INDIVIDUAL LEARNER DIFFERENCES ... 9

1.2.1. Cognitive variables ... 10

1.2.1.1. Intelligence ... 10

1.2.1.2. Aptitude ... 14

1.2.1.3. Cognitive styles and learning styles ... 26

1.2.1.4. Learning strategies ... 35 1.2.1.5. Age ... 41 1.2.2. Affective variables ... 44 1.2.2.1. Motivation ... 45 1.2.2.2. Personality ... 49 1.2.2.3. Anxiety ... 52 1.2.2.4. Self-esteem ... 53 1.2.2.5. Willingness to communicate ... 55 1.2.3. Social variables ... 56 1.2.3.1. Gender ... 56

(4)

1.2.3.2. Beliefs ... 58

CHAPTER 2 : COMPUTER ASSISTED LANGUAGE LEARNING ... 61

2.1.A DEFINITION OF CALL ... 62

2.2.A BRIEF HISTORY OF CALL ... 65

2.3.CALLDIVERSITY OF ENVIRONMENTS ... 70

2.3.1. Face-to-face environments ... 71

2.3.2. Blended environments ... 72

2.3.3. Distance environments ... 77

2.3.4. Virtual environments ... 79

2.4.WEB 2.0 APPLICATIONS IN FOREIGN LANGUAGE LEARNING ... 82

2.4.1. The Internet ... 85

2.4.2. Computer Mediated Communication ... 91

2.4.3. Concordances ... 94

2.4.4. Online dictionaries ... 97

2.4.5. Computer Aided Testing ... 101

2.5.RESEARCH DIRECTIONS INTO CALL ... 105

CHAPTER 3 : EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATIONS OF IDS AND CALL IN FL LEARNING ... 111

3.1.THE IMPORTANCE OF IDS IN CALL ... 112

3.2.COGNITIVE VARIABLES AND CALL ... 114

3.3.AFFECTIVE VARIABLES AND CALL... 126

CHAPTER 4 : METHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS ... 136

4.1.RESEARCH DESIGN ... 137

4.2.RESEARCH QUESTIONS ... 140

4.3.PARTICIPANTS ... 141

4.4.PILOT STUDY ... 144

4.4.1. Participants of the pilot study ... 145

4.4.2. Instruments used in the pilot study ... 145

4.5.INSTRUMENTS ... 149

4.5.1. Measures of L2 proficiency ... 149

(5)

4.5.1.2. Speaking and Writing Tasks ... 151

4.5.1.3. Pearson Longman Introductory Placement Test ... 153

4.5.2. Measures of Individual Differences and CALL ... 154

4.5.2.1. Strategy Inventory for Language Learning (SILL) ... 154

4.5.2.2. Learning Style Survey (LSS) ... 156

4.5.2.3. Motivation Battery ... 157

4.5.2.4. Foreign Language Aptitude TestPolish, TUNJO ... 159

4.5.2.5. The Learner Profile and the Beliefs about CALL Questionnaire ... 163

4.5.2.6. Interviews with the students ... 164

4.6.PROCEDURES ... 166

4.6.1. The experimental group ... 168

4.6.2. The control group ... 173

4.7.DATA ANALYSIS ... 174 4.7.1. Quantitative analysis ... 174 4.7.1.1. Assumptions ... 175 4.7.1.2. Descriptive statistics ... 183 4.7.1.3. Correlation ... 183 4.7.1.4. Multiple regression ... 184

4.7.1.5. Independent and paired-samples t-tests ... 185

4.7.1.6. Statistical significance ... 185

4.7.2. Qualitative data analysis ... 186

CHAPTER 5 : FINDINGS OF THE RESEARCH PROJECT ... 188

5.1.INDIVIDUAL VARIABLES ... 189

5.1.1. Language learning strategies ... 189

5.1.2. Learning style preferences ... 192

5.1.3. Motivation ... 195

5.1.4. Foreign language aptitude ... 201

5.2.COMPUTER ASSISTED LANGUAGE LEARNING ... 204

5.2.1. The Beliefs about CALL questionnaire ... 204

5.2.2. Interviews with the E-learners ... 208

5.3.RESULTS OF PROFICIENCY TESTS ... 217

(6)

5.3.2. Speaking tasks ... 220

5.3.3. Writing tasks ... 222

5.4.RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN VARIABLES ... 224

5.4.1. The outcomes of the correlation analysis ... 224

5.4.2. Multiple regression ... 228 5.5.DISCUSSION ... 231 CONCLUSION ... 242 SUMMARY ... 247 REFERENCES ... 249 APPENDIX A ... 287 APPENDIX B ... 292 APPENDIX C ... 296 APPENDIX D ... 303

(7)

List of tables

Table 1.1. Taxonomies of individual learner differences. ... 6

Table 1.2. The seven intelligences (adapted from Gardner and Hatch 1989). ... 13

Table 1.3. A comparison of the MLAT and the PLAB (after Stern 1994). ... 15

Table 1.4. SLA processing stages and potential aptitude components (from Skehan 2002). ... 19

Table 1.5. Kolb’s model of learning styles (based on Kolb and Kolb 2005a). ... 28

Table 1.6. Definitions of learning strategiesan overview. ... 36

Table 2.1. Selected terms related to CALL, based on: Ahmad et al. (1985), Beatty (2010), Stockwell (2012), and Gruba (2004). ... 63

Table 2.2. The three stages of CALL (adapted from Warschauer 2011). ... 68

Table 2.3. Restricted, Open and Integrated CALL: an outline (from Bax 2003). ... 69

Table 2.4. Course classifications (adapted from Allen et al. 2007). ... 73

Table 2.5. Similarities and differences between traditional learning, e-learning, and blended learning, adapted from Olejarczuk (2014a). ... 74

Table 3.1. Review of selected studies on IDs and CALL presented chronologically. . 113

Table 3.2. MI enhanced by CALL activities (adapted from Kim 2009). ... 115

Table 4.1. Chronology of the research schedule. ... 138

Table 4.2. Participants of the study. ... 142

Table 4.3. BULATStypes of listening tasks (adapted from “BULATS: Information for candidates” 2011). ... 150

Table 4.4. BULATStypes of reading and language knowledge tasks, part 1 (adapted from “BULATS: Information for candidates” 2011). ... 150

Table 4.5. BULATStypes of reading and language knowledge tasks, part 2 (adapted from “BULATS: Information for candidates” 2011). ... 151

(8)

Table 4.6. Cronbach’s Alpha for most recent versions of Standard BULATS test by

component and as a whole (adapted from BULATS Test Specification 2010). .... 151

Table 4.7. Sample items of the Pearson Longman Introductory Placement Test. ... 153

Table 4.8. Scoring for levels of the Pearson Longman Introductory Placement Test. . 153

Table 4.9. Sample items for Oxford’s (1989) Strategy Inventory for Language Learning. ... 155

Table 4.10. Sample items for Cohen et al.’s (2002) Learning Style Survey. ... 156

Table 4.11. Factors measured by the Motivation Battery. ... 158

Table 4.12. Comparison of the MLAT and the TUNJO (from Rysiewicz 2011). ... 160

Table 4.13. TUNJOtest items. ... 160

Table 4.14. Cronbach’s alpha for the TUNJO (adapted from Rysiewicz 2008a). ... 163

Table 4.15. Interview items. ... 165

Table 4.16. Course syllabus for all the groups according to group symbol. ... 167

Table 4.17. The procedure used in the experimental group. ... 169

Table 4.18. A syllabus for the Business English component of the course. ... 170

Table 4.19. The procedure used in the control group. ... 174

Table 4.20. Outliers, Mean and 5% Trimmed Mean scores for the independent variables. ... 177

Table 4.21. Skewness and kurtosis values. ... 178

Table 4.22. Collinearity statistics for Bulats0 and Bulats0A as dependent variables. . 182

Table 4.23. Collinearity statistics for Speaking Task 1 and Speaking Task 2 as dependent variables. ... 182

Table 4.24. Collinearity statistics for Writing Task 1 and Writing Task 2 as dependent variables. ... 182

Table 5.1. Descriptive statistics for the SILL. ... 189

Table 5.2. Descriptive statistics for the LSS. ... 193

Table 5.3. Descriptive statistics for the MB questionnaire according to factors of the L2 self. ... 196

Table 5.4. Descriptive statistics for the MB questionnaire. ... 198

Table 5.5. Descriptive statistics for the TUNJO. ... 201

Table 5.6. Descriptive statistics for the Beliefs about CALL Questionnaire. ... 204

Table 5.7. Descriptive statistics for Bulats0. ... 218

(9)

Table 5.9. The means and independent samples t-tests for the BULATS test (between

groups)... 219

Table 5.10. The means and paired samples t-tests for the BULATS test. ... 220

Table 5.11. Descriptive statistics for Speaking Tasks 1-2. ... 221

Table 5.12. The means and independent samples t-tests for the Speaking Task (between groups)... 221

Table 5.13. The means and paired samples t-tests for the Speaking Task (within groups). ... 221

Table 5.14. Descriptive statistics for Writing Tasks 1-2. ... 222

Table 5.15. The means and independent samples t-tests for the Writing Task (between groups)... 223

Table 5.16. The means and paired samples t-tests for the Writing Task. ... 223

Table 5.17. Correlations for all the variables in the experimental group. ... 225

Table 5.18. Correlations for particular SILL components in the experimental group. . 227

Table 5.19. A summary of multiple regression analysis for the Bulats0. ... 229

Table 5.20. A summary of multiple regression analysis for the Bulats0A. ... 229

Table 5.21. A summary of multiple regression analysis for the Sp1. ... 229

Table 5.22. A summary of multiple regression analysis for the Sp2. ... 230

Table 5.23. A summary of multiple regression analysis for the Wr1. ... 230

(10)

List of figures

Fig. 1.1. A hierarchical model of aptitude complexes, ability factors and cognitive abilities

(from Robinson 2007). ... 20

Fig. 1.2. The two dimensions of cognitive style (from Riding and Sadler-Smith 1997).32 Fig. 1.3. Oxford’s (1990a) taxonomy of learning strategies. ... 38

Fig. 2.1. Blended learning possibilities (adapted from Bath and Bourke 2010). ... 75

Fig. 2.2. A virtual seabed in Second Life. ... 82

Fig. 2.3. Lingu@network website. ... 87

Fig. 2.4. News in Levels website. ... 89

Fig. 2.5. Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA). ... 96

Fig. 2.6. Cambridge Free English Dictionary and Thesaurus online. ... 98

Fig. 2.7. Visual Dictionary Online. ... 100

Fig. 2.8. Computer adaptive testing procedure (adapted from Linacre 2000). ... 102

Fig. 3.1. Perspectives of computer help seen as similar to that from another person (A) or as distinct from the help of another person (B) (adapted from Chapelle and Heift 2009). ... 119

Fig. 4.1. Male and female participants of the study. ... 141

Fig. 4.2. Number of participants in groups. ... 142

Fig. 4.3. Survey codebook for the Learner Profile. ... 146

Fig. 4.4. A sample unit in the online course. ... 171

Fig. 4.5. A sample interactive exercise. ... 172

Fig. 4.6. A sample boxplot presenting a detected outlier in the current study. ... 176

Fig. 4.7. A sample histogram presenting normally distributed data for the CALL questionnaire. ... 179

(11)

Fig. 4.8. A sample q-q plot presenting normally distributed data for the CALL

questionnaire. ... 179

Fig. 4.9. A matrix of scatterplots. ... 180

Fig. 5.1. Language learning strategy use according to strategy group. ... 190

Fig. 5.2. Motivation level according to factors in the L2 self. ... 197

Fig. 5.3. Average aptitude profiles. ... 203

Fig. 5.4. CALL questionnaire mean scores. ... 207

Fig. 5.5. Bulats tests results. ... 219

Fig. 5.6. A scatterplot presenting correlation between the SILL and the CALL in the experimental group. ... 225

Fig. 5.7. A scatterplot presenting correlation between the MB and the LSS in the experimental group. ... 226

Fig. 5.8. A scatterplot presenting correlation between the SILL and the CALL in the experimental group. ... 226

(12)

Introduction

It is beyond doubt that learning a foreign language is a complex phenomenon and that some people learn faster achieving more spectacular results than others, which is aptly noted by Segalowitz (1997: 85), who claims: “Why do individuals differ so much in second lan-guage (L2) attainment? After all, every healthy human being in an intact social environ-ment masters a first language to a degree of fluency that, in other skill domains, would be recognized as elite or near elite levels (…)”. Therefore, in the past decades a number of applied linguists have invested a large amount of effort into trying to identify, name, classi-fy, and describe the individual learner variables in respect of which people differ in order to examine what accounts for learners’ differential success in foreign language (FL) learning. This was summarized by Larsen-Freeman and Long (1994: 153), who state: “one of the major conundrums in the SLA field is the question of differential success”. These individu-al learner factors have been classified into the following broad categories: cognitive, affec-tive, and social (Pawlak 2012a). Although numerous scholars give priority to learners’ mo-tivation (Dörnyei 2005a) or foreign language aptitude (Rysiewicz 2004) as factors determining the ultimate achievement, quite a few experts also share the present research-er’s opinion that such variables as language learning strategies or learning styles prefer-ences may shape the trajectories of FL learning and, therefore, should not be ignored.

It is also true that ubiquitous computing and round-the-clock access to the Internet, which provides a great number of web 2.0 tools, have opened new horizons for FL learning and teaching and increased the need for teacher training and professional development. With the advent of modern technologies and new ways of learning that were unknown sev-eral years ago, it is clearly interesting to investigate learner individual differences (IDs) in different computer-assisted FL learning environments, such as face-to-face, distance, virtu-al or blended learning. Additionvirtu-ally, because of the fact that, obviously, students learn in a variety of different ways and no single methodology is effective for all of them, it would be

(13)

useful to look at learners’ beliefs about Computer Assisted Language Learning (CALL) and types of instruction that they are likely to benefit from. Although research on individual learner differences has proliferated in the last decades and the area of CALL has received increased attention in the past few years, there are only few extensive, state-of-the-art stud-ies conducted among English for Specific Purposes students in which FL learning was aid-ed by CALL. Thus, the study reportaid-ed in the current dissertation is an attempt to fill the existing gap and dispel some of the myths surrounding the place of the computer medium in the EFL classroom. What is more, the topic appears to be fascinating and challenging at the same time. It should be also emphasized that the research project discussed in this dis-sertation largely stems from the present researcher’s own experience as a language learner and a foreign language teacher.

It is interesting to note that this research project, exploring the relationship between IDs and CALL, is significant for several reasons. First of all, it may help EFL curricula designers and methodologists develop teaching materials which would suit various ways of teaching and learning and match students’ level of L2 achievement. Moreover, this study may aid students by helping them to learn in a more enjoyable and effective manner by, for example, using an array of language learning strategies. Furthermore, this research project may encourage other researchers to conduct further studies on the same topic. Finally, the outcomes of the study will extend Polish and international literature on the influence of individual learner variables on FL attainment aided by CALL. In this dissertation, an at-tempt is made to explore the impact of selected cognitive and affective characteristics on ESP technical university students’ achievement in a blended learning environment. In addi-tion, the effectiveness of the two types of instrucaddi-tion, i.e. face-to-face and blended learning is investigated with respect to learners’ beliefs about CALL. These objectives are achieved by employing a mixed methods research approach, which reconciles both quantitative and qualitative data collection procedures.

The current dissertation consists of five chapters, of which the first three are intend-ed as a review of relevant theoretical background and the remaining ones present and dis-cuss methodological considerations and findings of the empirical investigation. To be more precise, Chapter 1 explains basic terms in individual learner differences research and pre-sents definitions, conceptualizations and classifications of the leading IDs, with particular attention being given to the factors that are the main focus of the current thesis, i.e. lan-guage learning strategies, learning styles, foreign lanlan-guage aptitude, and motivation.

(14)

Chapter 2, in turn, meant as an overview of the key issues related to Computer Assisted Language Learning, focuses on the definitions of CALL adopted for the purpose of this work and the vital distinctions between various CALL environments. A separate section is devoted to the importance of different CALL applications, or ways of harnessing computers for the purpose of FL learning, which is followed by a discussion of the main research di-rections into CALL. In Chapter 3, the emphasis is shifted to empirical investigations of the relationship between distinct IDs and CALL by first outlining a framework for conducting such studies, and subsequently presenting and discussing their methodology and main find-ings with respect to the effectiveness of specific types of instruction, the software used, and other mediating variables. The focus of attention in Chapter 4 is on the methodological considerations related to the study described in this thesis. This chapter includes infor-mation concerning the design, participants of the study as well as procedures applied throughout the process of data collection, data analysis and the interpretation of the results. Finally, Chapter 5 reports the findings of the research project, with the analysis and discus-sion of the results being followed by a set of tentative suggestions which, in the opinion of the author, could prove useful to foreign language teachers and instructors. The thesis clos-es with a conclusion that offers a summary of the most vital points touched upon through-out this dissertation, provides a set of pedagogical implications for FL teachers and re-searchers, and considers the possible objectives of future research endeavours and the ways in which these can be pursued. Being fully aware of the limitations of the current study and the tentative nature of the pedagogical recommendations, the present researcher hopes that these practicable solutions will contribute to increased efficiency of CALL-aided foreign language instruction among learners who aim at becoming competent users of English as a foreign language.

(15)

Chapter 1: Individual Differences in Second Language

Acqui-sition

Introduction

There is a plethora of unique differences that account for an individual’s success in the learning of a second/foreign1 language. The study of language learner characteristics in

respect of which people differ has a long tradition in foreign language studies and nobody would undermine the importance of such factors as motivation or aptitude. Accordingly, there is a vast number of articles and books devoted to this topic. The present chapter be-gins with an attempt to provide definitions of IDs, outlining different taxonomies proposed by various authors. For the purpose of this work, individual differences are divided into three broad categories of cognitive, affective, and social variables. Due to the fact that the scope of this chapter is limited, the discussion will mainly be confined to such factors as: intelligence, aptitude, cognitive/learning styles, learning strategies, age, motivation, per-sonality, anxiety, self-esteem, willingness to communicate, as well as gender and beliefs. The selection of the IDs is dictated by the present author’s belief that these particular varia-bles appear to be the most promising areas of research in the field of Second Language Ac-quisition (SLA) and several of them were the focus of the study reported in the empirical part of the dissertation.

(16)

1.1. Definitions and taxonomies of individual differences (IDs)

It is a common observation that people differ from each other, yet it is less obvious why and how they differ. The field of study that deals with individual and group differences in human behaviour is called differential psychology. As Revelle et al. (2011: 3) summarized in the Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences, “The study of individual dif-ferences include the study of affect, behavior, cognition and motivation as they are affected by biological causes and environmental events”. Furthermore, researchers in the field of SLA have been interested in the individual differences between people learning their sec-ond, third or even fourth foreign language. A number of psychologists and applied linguists have made attempts to define, describe and classify individual differences in order to iden-tify factors that account for success in learning a second/foreign language. As Cohen (2010: 161) explains, “When students embark on the study of an L2, they are not merely ‘empty vessels’ that will need to be filled by the wise words of the teacher; instead, they carry a considerable ‘personal baggage’ to the language course that will have a significant bearing on how learning proceeds”. Indeed, a handful of factors of the learner’s ‘baggage’ can po-tentially affect success in foreign language learning. Among them, there are variables that are relatively easily identifiable, such as age or gender, and those that are much more diffi-cult to grasp, mainly due to problems involved in their measurement, such as intelligence, aptitude, motivation, learning styles, learning strategies or personality factors.

It is interesting to note that it is not very complicated to find definitions of individu-al differences in the literature. Strelau (2006) explains that the notion of individuindividu-al differ-ences is connected with the fact that entities, both human beings and animals, that belong to the same population are different in respect of comparable physical and mental characteris-tics. Dörnyei (2005a: 1), in turn, argues, “As the term suggests, individual differences (IDs) are characteristics or traits in respect of which individuals may be shown to differ from each other”. In another of Dörnyei’s (2006: 42) publications, he explains, “Individual dif-ferences (IDs) refer to dimensions of enduring personal characteristics that are assumed to apply to everybody and on which people differ by degree. In other words, they concern stable and systematic deviations from a normative blueprint”. Even though the definitions presented above do not seem to be controversial, some problems arise with particular indi-vidual variablesfor example, applied linguists have not been able to reach a consensus on whether learning styles can be equated with cognitive styles or to what extent intelligence is a part of foreign language aptitude. This brings about a problem with taxonomies of

(17)

indi-vidual differences which have been provided by various researchers according to differing criteria. As Ellis (1985: 10) claims, “The learner factors that can influence the course of development are potentially infinite and very difficult to classify in a reliable manner”. Ta-ble 1.1. presents an overview of selected classifications of individual learner differences in chronological order proposed by different researchers. As can be seen from this list, classi-fications of individual variables have proved to be problematic as different scholars focus on various characteristics, which then are grouped into separate categories. For example, Dörnyei (2006) enumerates five most important ID variables while Cohen (2010) suggests only two categories which embrace many factors.

Table 1.1. Taxonomies of individual learner differences.

researcher taxonomy

Ellis (1985) personal factors and general factors

Gardner (1985) language aptitude, personality, attitudes, motivation and orientation

Cook (1991) motivation, aptitude, learning strategies, age, personality, other indi-vidual variation Larsen-Freeman and Long (1994) age, aptitude, socio-psychological factors, personality, cognitive style, hemisphere specialization, learning strategies, other factors Williams and Burden (1997) intelligence, cognitive style, motivation, anxiety, aptitude and learning strategies Brown (2000) styles and strategies, personality factors, sociocultural factors, age, aptitude and intelligence

Ehrman et al. (2003) learning styles, learning strategies and affective variables

Dörnyei and Skehan (2003) aptitude, cognitive and learning styles, learner strategies and moti-vation

Ellis (2004) abilities, propensities, learner cognitions about L2 learning and learner actions

Dörnyei (2006) personality, aptitude, motivation, learning styles and learning strategies

Johnson (2008) cognitive variables, affective variables, personality variables and learn-ing strategies Pawlak (2009) age, intelligence, aptitude, cognitive and learning styles, learning strat-egies, motivation, anxiety, beliefs and willingness to communicate Cohen (2010) characteristics outside the teacher’s control and characteristics that can be shaped during the process of second language learning

Starting with the taxonomy proposed by Ellis (1985), one can observe that individu-al differences are divided into two broad categories: personindividu-al and generindividu-al factors. The for-mer refer to individuals’ L2 learning and include nesting patterns, transition anxiety and the desire to maintain a personal language learning agenda. The latter were further divided into modifiable and unmodifiable factors and, as the name suggests, modifiable factors are those

(18)

than can be changed during the course of Second Language Acquisition, e.g. motivation, and unmodifiable factors are those which cannot be manipulated to some extent, e.g. apti-tude. Ellis also concludes that there are social, cognitive and affective aspects of both per-sonal and general factors. Gardner (1985), in turn, chooses to discuss language aptitude together with personality in one chapter and attitudes with motivation in another, and treats orientation as a distinct concept from motivation dividing it into two types: integrative and instrumental. Such a selection of IDs was probably dictated by Gardner’s interest in those individual learner factors, motivation in particular. Cook (1991) provides a selection of individual differences singling out the following variables: motivation, aptitude, learning strategies, age, personality and other individual variation. What is surprising in this taxon-omy is the fact that he only briefly discusses the issue of cognitive style as well as the in-trovert/extrovert distinction under the label of personality, and uses the term other individ-ual variation for such factors as intelligence, sex differences, command of the first language or empathy, providing only a very brief sentence description for each of the vari-ables. Larsen-Freeman and Long (1994) devote one chapter, entitled Explanations for dif-ferential success among second language learners, to eight IDs categories. They further divide some of the sections into the following subsections: socio-psychological factors into motivation and attitude; personality into self-esteem, extroversion, anxiety, risk-taking, sensitivity to rejection, empathy, inhibition, tolerance of ambiguity; and cognitive style into field independence/dependence category width, reflectivity, impulsivity, aural/visual, and analytic/gestalt. Finally, they discuss six IDs which they label other factors, that is (1) memory, awareness, will, (2) language disability, (3) interest, (4) sex, (5) birth order, and (6) prior experience.

Williams and Burden (1997), first of all, divide individual differences into two cate-gories: obvious (age, gender, personality, aptitude, intelligence and motivation) and less obvious (cognitive styles and strategies, anxiety and preparedness to take risks). Secondly, they briefly describe such individual variables as intelligence, cognitive style, motivation, anxiety, aptitude, learning strategies and, finally, decide to devote two whole chapters to motivation and learning strategies. Brown (2000) makes a distinction between styles and strategies, personality factors (self-esteem, inhibition, risk-taking, anxiety, empathy, extro-version/introversion, and motivation), sociocultural factors, age, aptitude and intelligence, and discusses them in separate chapters. It is interesting to note that he views motivation as a personality factor, which might be regarded as a somewhat surprising idea. Ehrman et al. (2003) focus their attention on learning styles, learning strategies and affective variables,

(19)

i.e. motivation, self-efficacy, tolerance of ambiguity, and anxiety. They also mention other areas of individual differences, such as aptitude, gender, culture, age and other demograph-ic variables. Dörnyei and Skehan (2003) organize their artdemograph-icle on IDs in SLA into four main sections: foreign language aptitude, cognitive and learning styles, learner strategies, and motivation, deciding at the same time to omit some individual variables, not describing, for example, personality. Another scholar providing a taxonomy of factors responsible for in-dividual differences in L2 learning is Ellis (2004), who divides them into four categories: abilities, propensities, learner cognitions about L2 learning and learner actions. The first category, that is abilities, refers to cognitive capabilities for language learning and com-prises such factors as intelligence, language aptitude and memory. Propensities can be de-fined as cognitive and affective qualities, such as learning style, motivation, anxiety, per-sonality, and willingness to communicate. Learner cognitions about L2 learning include learner beliefs and learner actions equated with learning strategies. Ellis does not describe some of the other important individual variables, among which age is the most conspicuous,

justifying his decision by the fact that the four main categories do not comprise age; they are rather affected by it. He also implies that age is too broad an area and requires separate treatment. Dörnyei (2006), in turn, introduces an overview of five individual factors which comprise personality, aptitude, motivation, learning styles and learning strategies, all of which he sees as the most important ID variables. Johnson (2008) in his introductory course to foreign language learning and teaching groups individual differences into cognitive vari-ables (intelligence and aptitude), affective varivari-ables (motivation and attitude), personality variables (extroversion/introversion, tolerance of ambiguity, empathy or ego permeability, and cognitive style), and learning strategies. This taxonomy appears to be incomplete, tak-ing into consideration the fact that the scholar does not mention some important factors, the most notable of which is age.

Two of the recent individual differences taxonomies have been proposed by Pawlak (2009) and Cohen (2010). Pawlak (2009) confines his discussion to the following IDs: age, intelligence, aptitude, cognitive and learning styles, learning strategies, motivation, anxiety, beliefs and willingness to communicate, which are grouped into four categories. Firstly, he describes age, intelligence, and aptitude, which he regards as cognitive in nature and makes the comment that such factors cannot be controlled by the teacher or the learner. Secondly, he enumerates cognitive styles, learning styles and learning strategies which are, as is the case of age, intelligence and aptitude, cognitive in nature, but can be manipulated externally to some extent. Thirdly, the scholar focuses on motivation, which is clearly subject to

(20)

change and, finally, he discusses anxiety, beliefs and willingness to communicate. Cohen (2010) elects to focus on individual characteristics outside the teacher’s control and such factors that can be shaped during the process of second language learning. Among the for-mer he includes age, gender and language aptitude; whereas the latter comprise learning styles, learning strategies and motivation, which, in his view, are interrelated in a variety of ways.

It should be stated that many of the variables mentioned above cannot be affected by the teacher or are generally considered to be stable factors, among them: age, gender, aptitude, intelligence or some personality traits. There are, however, factors that can be shaped to some extent through appropriate training, e.g. learning strategies or motivation, in order to help learners achieve better results in foreign language learning. All things con-sidered, it should be stated that further attempts should be made in order to provide one inclusive taxonomy of individual learner differences.

1.2. Overview of selected individual learner differences

In the present thesis, the individual learner differences are classified into three broad cate-gories: cognitive, affective, and social variables. While a variety of definitions of the aforementioned terms have been suggested, this dissertation will use the definition pro-posed by Ellis (1985: 100):

Social aspects are external to the learner and concern the relationship between the learner and native speakers of the L2 and also between the learner and other speakers of his own lan-guage. Cognitive and affective aspects are internal to the learner. Cognitive factors concern the nature of the problem-solving strategies used by the learner, while affective factors con-cern the emotional responses aroused by the attempts to learn a L2.

The subsequent parts of subsection 1.2. will be devoted to describing the three categories of individual variables with more emphasis being laid on the factors that are the main focus of the empirical investigation reported in the present thesis.

(21)

1.2.1. Cognitive variables

There are two sets of intellectual qualities connected with second or foreign language learn-ing, both of which are cognitive in nature. The first one, more general, is intelligence and the other one is aptitude that specifically refers to learning a language. Therefore, this sec-tion will begin with a descripsec-tion of these two individual learner differences, followed by a discussion of other cognitive variables such as cognitive styles and learning styles, learning strategies, and age.

1.2.1.1. Intelligence

The field of intelligence research has been of great interest to numerous researchers over literally hundreds of years, mainly due to the fact that this topic appears to be inherently fascinating. However, there has been considerable disagreement concerning the definition and structure of intelligence because, as Sternberg (1985: 3) points out, “Intelligence is among the most elusive of concepts”. Williams and Burden (1997: 17) claim, “Intelligence is a topic about which a great deal has been written, but about which most teachers continue to feel confused”.

In fact, although the area of intelligence has been widely researched and a great number of definitions have been proposed, there is not one universally accepted definition of the concept. Ellis (1985: 110), for example, defines intelligence in the following way: “Intelligence is the term used to refer to a hypothesized ‘general factor’ (often referred to as the ‘g’ factor), which underlies our ability to master and use a whole range of academic skills”. Brown (2000: 100) argues, “Intelligence has traditionally been defined and meas-ured in terms of linguistic and logical-mathematical abilities”. Lightbown and Spada (2006: 57), in turn, explain that, “The term ‘intelligence’ has traditionally been used to refer to performance on certain kinds of tests”. Komorowska (2009) maintains that the concept of intelligence is not narrowly defined yet and that intelligence is often connected with the ability to cope with a new situation. Dörnyei (2005a) distinguishes between ability, aptitude and intelligence and claims that intelligence is a synonym for the first of these. Williams and Burden (1997: 19) summarize the introductory chapter to their book on individual dif-ferences in the following way: “Intelligence is what psychologists call a hypothetical

(22)

con-struct, a term of convenience to account for something that doesn’t really exist”. Finally, an overview of intelligence definitions by different authors and sources is offered by Legg and Hutter (2007: 22), who come up with their own, informal definition of the notion, which is: “Intelligence measures an agent’s ability to achieve goals in a wide range of environ-ments”.

In addition to trying to define the concept, attempts have been made to find out how to measure intelligence and what components it consists of. An important scholar who con-tributed to intelligence research development was William Stern, a German psychologist and philosopher, who coined the term Intelligence Quotient (IQ). At the beginning of the 20th century, intelligence was thought to be a factor contributing to foreign language learn-ing to a large extent. The question was if high scores on IQ tests could predict success or failure in all four language skills, i.e. listening, speaking, reading and writing to the same extent. When immersion programmes started to be developed in Canada, Genesee (1976) attempted to find out whether those programmes could be equally successful with both highly intelligent and less intelligent children. He correlated French achievement tests and intelligence test scores; however, he did not find any relationship between intelligence and what he described as communication skills, i.e. speaking and listening. By contrast, he did find a relationship between intelligence and what he labelled as academic language skills  reading and writing. Genesee’s finding led to a conclusion that intelligence is related to foreign language learning in respect of certain language skills. A few years later, Cummins (1980) made a distinction between what he defined as basic interpersonal communicative skills (BICS) and cognitive/academic language proficiency (CALP), and found out that IQ test scores were related to the latter.

As mentioned earlier, attempts have been made to describe what components intel-ligence includes. As Sternberg (1996: 11) comments, “On no question about intelintel-ligence has there been greater disagreement among psychologists than on the question of its struc-ture. Undoubtedly, describing all theories of intelligence in detail would go beyond the scope of this chapter. Therefore, only the best-known ones will be briefly described. It seems reasonable to start the discussion with a British educational psychologist, Charles Spearman (1904), who claimed that intelligence is composed of two kinds of factors: spe-cific factors (s), unique to the tasks used to measure intelligence, and a general factor (g) common to all meaningful activities, the latter of which “has generated considerable con-troversy” (Kane and Brand 2003: 7). Thurstone (1938) believed in the existence of the

(23)

fol-lowing seven primary mental abilities: verbal comprehension, word fluency, number facili-ty, special visualization, associative memory, perceptual speed and reasoning. Guilford (1967) distinguished three components of intelligence which are: operations (five kinds), contents (five kinds), and products (six kinds). Due to the fact that all three subcategories are independent, they are multiplicative and, as a consequence, they form a large cube of 150 different factors. Another conceptualization of intelligence was presented by Goleman (1995), who named it emotional intelligence, in which emotion is placed at the higher level of human abilities hierarchy.

Yet another theory of intelligence that was proposed was a theory of fluid intelli-gence and crystallized intelliintelli-gence. Its author Raymond B. Cattell (1967: 209) argued:

According to the theory of fluid and crystallized general ability, there is not one general abil-ity factor, as originally propounded by Spearman (1904) and supported by Thurstone (1938), but two. It states that these two broad factors are distinguishable by one, crystallized intelli-gence, gc, loading most heavily the culturally acquired judgmental skills, while the other, called fluid ability, gf, is found loading insightful performances in which individual differ-ences in learning experience play little part.

According to Cattell, the famous/infamous g factor of intelligence is composed of two abil-ities: fluid and crystallized. The former involves the ability to reason and solve problems; whereas the latter refers to knowledge and skills that are accumulated over a lifetime, and tends to increase with age. As can be seen above, there were researchers who distinguished only two components of intelligence (Spearman 1904) and those who postulated the exist-ence of as many as 150 of them (Guilford 1967). As Gardner and Moran (2006: 227) put it succinctly, “the debate of whether intelligence is a singular individual quality or a plethora of components (…) has waxed and waned throughout the 20th century”.

Apart from the general intelligence theories briefly discussed earlier, somewhat more promising perhaps appear to be Gardner’s (1983) Theory of Multiple Intelligences and Sternberg’s (1985) Theory of Successful Intelligence, which are specifically related to language learning. Gardner, a Harvard psychologist, argued that instead of viewing intelli-gence as a unitary construct, it should be considered as being composed of different kinds of intelligences. The theory that he advanced was revolutionary at the time it was proposed and, as Gardner (1983: 5) explains, “[It] challenges the classical view of intelligence that most of us have absorbed explicitly (from psychology or education texts) or implicitly (by living in a culture with strong but possibly circumstanced view of intelligence)”. Gardner

(24)

listed seven intelligences, which are presented in Table 1.2., arguing that all people possess all of these intelligences; however, different types of intelligence predominate in different individuals.

Table 1.2. The seven intelligences (adapted from Gardner and Hatch 1989). The seven intelligences

Intelligence End-states Core components

Logical-mathematical Mathematician Scientist Sensitivity to, and capacity to discern, logical or numeri-cal patterns; ability to handle long chains of reasoning.

Linguistic Poet

Journalist Sensitivity to the sounds, rhythms, and meanings of words; sensitivity to the different functions of language.

Musical Composer

Violonist Abilities to produce and appreciate rhythm, pitch, and timbre; appreciation of the forms of musical expressive-ness.

Spatial Navigator

Sculptor Capacities to perceive the visual-spatial world accurately and to perform transformations on one’s initial percep-tions.

Bodilykinesthetic Dancer

Athlete Abilities to control one’s body movements and to handle objects skillfully.

Interpersonal Therapist

Salesman Capacities to discern and respond appropriately to the moods, temperaments, motivations, and desires of other people.

Intrapersonal Person with

de-tailed, accurate self-knowledge

Access to one’s own feelings and the ability to discrimi-nate among them to guide behavior; knowledge of one’s own strengths, weaknesses, desires, and intelligences.

Sternberg (1985), on the other hand, proposed a triarchic, or three-part theory of intelligence, which is referred to as a Theory of Successful Intelligence. Sternberg (2005: 189) suggested the following multidimensional definition of successful intelligence, ac-cording to which “(Successful) intelligence is 1) the ability to achieve one’s goals in life, given one’s sociocultural context; 2) by capitalizing on strengths and correcting or compen-sating for weaknesses; 3) in order to adapt to, shape, and select environments; and 4) through a combination of analytical, creative, and practical abilities”. Consequently, as the name three-part theory suggests, there are three major sets of components or mental pro-cesses underlying all aspects of intelligence: metacomponents (or executive propro-cesses), performance components, and knowledge acquisition components (Sternberg 2002b). Met-acomponents enable a person to plan and make decisions as well as monitor and evaluate the decision-making process, performance components execute the instructions of the met-acomponents, whereas knowledge acquisition components are all the processes responsible for acquiring new knowledge, such as selecting information. Sternberg and Grigorenko

(25)

(2003) underline three aspects of intelligence which are analytical abilities (involved in analyzing, evaluating and contrasting things), creative abilities (involved in creating, ex-ploring and discovering), and practical abilities (involved in implementing and putting into practice). It was also assumed that it is possible to ‘teach for’ successful intelligence (Sternberg 2002a; Sternberg and Grigorenko 2003; Sternberg and Grigorenko 2004), which involves instructing learners as well as assessing them analytically, creatively and practical-ly. This was connected with the belief that such teaching can enable students to recognize their strengths and compensate for their potential weaknesses.

To conclude, intelligence may be a powerful predictor of success in SLA, especially academic skills. However, there are still some issues connected with intelligence which have not been satisfactorily resolved yet. There is no universal definition of intelligence or a theory that would explicitly provide all the components of intelligence. There are also questions connected with whether intelligence is inborn or whether it can be modified. Without doubt, the notion of intelligence has been connected with success at learning. Con-sequently, researchers have attempted to conceptualize the ability that is connected with success in foreign or second language learning, which will be discussed in section 1.2.1.2.

1.2.1.2. Aptitude

Closely related to intelligence is another cognitive individual variablelanguage aptitude. As Dörnyei (2006: 45) writes, “The concept of language aptitude is related to the broader concept of human abilities, or intelligence, covering a variety of cognitively-based learner differences”. Language aptitude has been referred to in different ways, such as, for instance a ‘special ability’, ‘gift’, ‘knack’, ‘feel’, or ‘flair’ for languages (Cohen 2010) or special ‘propensity’ or ‘talent’ for learning an L2 (Dörnyei 2005a). As Stern (1994: 368) explains, “The definition of second language aptitude and its measurement depend upon underlying language teaching theories and interpretations of learner characteristics and of the language learning process”. Lightbown and Spada (2006: 57), for example, define language learning aptitude as “specific abilities thought to predict success in language learning (…)”. Ranta (2002: 162), in turn, claims, “Aptitude is viewed as a stable trait of the individual which predicts how quickly he or she will learn a foreign language”. A more recent definition of L2 aptitude has been proposed by Robinson (2012: 57), who defined it as “the ability to

(26)

successfully adapt to and profit from instructed, or naturalistic exposure to the L2”. It is important to underline that language aptitude does not determine whether or not a person can learn a language but it can provide information on the rate at which he or she is likely to master an L2, an assumption that is supported by Dahlen and Caldwell-Harris (2013: 902), who claim, “It is commonly assumed that all typically developing individuals can learn a foreign language. However, the amount of time required and the best teaching method of learning environment may differ from person to person”.

A number of questions have been posed in the history of aptitude research, among which the most frequently asked ones are as follows:

 “Is there a specific talent for learning languages? If yes, what is the structure of such a talent?” (Skehan 1998);

 “Should languages be taught to everybody or only to those who have sufficient aptitude? Should students with different aptitudes be placed into separate ‘streams’? Can aptitude be developed by training?” (Stern 1994);

 “Is such a talent innate? Is it relatively fixed? Is it amenable to training? Can such a talent be measured effectively?” (Dörnyei and Skehan 2003).

Surprisingly, although nobody questions the existence of aptitude, it is difficult to present one universal answer to all the questions listed above or provide an extensive description of the construct. It is reasonable to start the discussion of language aptitude with a description of two test development programmes implemented by John Carroll and Stanley Sapon as well as Paul Pimsleur, who designed two of the leading instruments of prognosis and diag-nosis in the 1950s and 1960s. The Modern Language Aptitude Test (MLAT; Carroll and Sapon 1959) and the Pimsleur Language Aptitude Battery (PLAB or LAB; Pimsleur 1966) have become the most widely used and cited aptitude tests from the time they were devel-oped (Dörnyei 2005a).

Table 1.3. A comparison of the MLAT and the PLAB (after Stern 1994). MLAT and PLAT constituents

MLAT PLAB

Test task descriptions

Ability assessed

Test task descriptions

Names of tests Names of tests

Learn words for num-bers in an artificial language.

Number learning Symbol discrimination Learn phonetic

distinc-tions and recognize them in different

(27)

con-texts. Listen to sounds and

learn phonetic symbols for them.

Phonetic script Sound-symbol

associa-tion Associate sounds with written symbols. Decipher phonetically

spelt English words and identify words with similar meanings.

Spelling clues Rhymes List as many words as

possible that rhyme with four given words. The ability to discriminate, remember, interpret,

and produce the phonic substance of another language. Auditory alertness. The ability to relate the phonology to forms of graphemic representa-tion.

Recognize the syntac-tic functions of words and phrases in sen-tences.

Words in sentences Language analysis Make judgements with

the help of translations about the meanings and rules of use of an un-known language. The ability to pay attention to morphological,

syntactic, and semantic features of a language, to relate linguistic forms to each other, and to de-velop patterns, regularities, and rules from lin-guistic materials: linlin-guistic (grammatical-semantic) sensitivity and an inductive learning ability.

Learn and recall words in an artificial lan-guage.

Number learning Paired associates

Memory ability: the capacity to memorize and recall words in a new language. Rote memory. MLAT only. Not tapped by PLAB.

Vocabulary Identify the meaning of

different words. Word knowledge, i.e., lexical competence in the

first language tested in PLAB only.

Grade-point average in

academic areas Information gathered by tester. Interest in learning

a foreign language Short questionnaire.

PLAB contains a general school achievement and motivational components, not considered in MLAT, as part of the concept of aptitude.

(28)

Table 1.3. presents a comparison of the MLAT and the PLAB which clearly shows that in both batteries language aptitude is not viewed as a single entity but a composite of different characteristics. The MLAT includes the following five parts: Number Learning, Phonetic Script, Spelling Clues, Words in Sentences and Paired Associates. According to Carroll (1965), there are four aptitude components which are:

1. Phonetic coding abilitythe ability to identify sounds, to form associations be-tween those sounds and symbols representing them as well as to retain those associ-ations.

2. Grammatical sensitivitythe ability to recognize the grammatical functions of words in sentences.

3. Rote learning abilitythe ability to form and remember associations between stim-uli.

4. Inductive language learning ability“the ability to infer linguistic forms, rules, and patterns from new linguistic content itself with a minimum of supervision or guid-ance” (Carroll 1965: 130).

The PLAB, on the other hand, is composed of the following six parts: Grade Point Aver-age, Interest in Foreign Language Learning, Vocabulary, Language Analysis, Sound Dis-crimination and Sound-Symbol Association that measure three factors of language aptitude proposed by Pimsleur which are as follows:

1. Verbal Intelligencethat comprises “(…) both knowledge of vocabulary in your native language and the ability to reason analytically about language (…)” (Pimsleur 1968: 73).

2. MotivationPimsleur (1966) claimed that motivation was significantly related to foreign language learning.

3. Auditory Abilitywhich is the ability to hear, recognize and reproduce sounds in a foreign language.

As can be observed in Table 1.3., there are striking similarities between the Modern Language Aptitude Test and the Pimsleur Language Aptitude Battery. Firstly, two of PLAB’s leading components deal with Verbal Intelligence and Auditory Ability, both of which have their counterparts in the MLAT. It is interesting to note that Pimsleur consid-ered verbal intelligence an important part of language aptitude and used two subtests, i.e. Vocabulary and Language Analysis to measure it. Carroll also devoted one subtest, that is Spelling Clues, to measuring verbal intelligence and vocabulary knowledge. Another

(29)

simi-larity is the fact that both tests were developed at a time when audiolingualism was the pre-vailing instructional approach (Ellis 2004). An obvious difference between these two in-struments was their respective authors’ stance on whether motivation is an integral part of aptitude (Pimsleur) or it is something which should not be measured using an aptitude test (Carroll). Another difference is the fact that the PLAB contains a general school achieve-ment component, which is not present in the MLAT. What is more, the PLAB was de-signed to be administered to a younger population than post-puberty or adult learners which is visible in the case of the MLAT test. Finally, the PLAB does not include any memory component.

Undoubtedly, the MLAT and the PLAB are two of the most widely used and re-ferred to language aptitude tests in the world (Dörnyei 2005a), also translated into other languages, e.g. into Hungarian (DeKeyser 2000; Sáfár and Kormos 2008). However, since the 1950s and 1960s when the MLAT and the PLAB were first used, a number of various studies have been conducted and there was therefore a need to update the theories and in-struments used to measure language aptitude (Robinson 2012; Robinson 2013). Interesting-ly, there are still some applied linguists who discount the relevance of aptitude, for example Cook (1991: 76), who claims:

Such tests are not neutral about what happens in the classroom, nor about the goals of lan-guage teaching. They assume that learning words by heart is an important part of L2 learning ability, that the spoken language is crucial, and that grammar consists of structural patterns. In short, MLAT mostly predicts howwell a student will do in a course that is predominantly audiolingual in methodology rather than in a course taught by other methods.

Cook’s point of view is justified by his conviction that the MLAT is not adequate in all teaching conditions and for all learners. Skehan (2002: 70) also calls into question the role of foreign language aptitude tests by stating, “(…) as instructional methodologies have changed, foreign language aptitude have been perceived as irrelevant”. Also with the grow-ing influence of the Communicative Language Teachgrow-ing (CLT) approach, the relevance of aptitude was questioned. To quote Skehan (2002: 72), “(…) aptitude was seen to be irrele-vant, and more appropriate to old-fashioned class learning”, especially in the light of SLA research growth after the 1970s as well as Krashen’s (1981) view that aptitude was relevant for learning, not acquisition.

However, as some experts point out, there has been “a notable reawakening of inter-est” (Ellis 2004: 533) in FL aptitude recently and “challenging reconceptualizations of apti-tude have emerged” (Dörnyei and Skehan 2003: 593). Similarly to Carroll’s and Pimsleur’s

(30)

belief that aptitude is componential, other researchers are also convinced that this cognitive variable is not a unitary construct (Skehan 2002; Skehan 1998; Sparks et al. 2011; Leaver et al. 2005; Sparks and Ganschow 1993; Parry and Child 1990; Rysiewicz 2003) and, as a consequence, other models of language aptitude have been proposed. Skehan (1998), for example, narrowed the model of foreign language aptitude down to the following three parts: Auditory ability, Linguistic ability and Memory ability. It is interesting to note that Skehan’s vision of FL aptitude simplifies that proposed by Carroll, since, for example, au-ditory ability corresponds to phonetic coding ability and memory ability is similar to rote learning ability. The third component proposed by Skehan, that is linguistic ability, com-prises Carroll’s inductive language learning ability and grammatical sensitivity.

In his most recent works, Skehan has attempted to relate various aptitude compo-nents to the different SLA process stages (Skehan 2002; Dörnyei and Skehan 2003), a pro-posal which is illustrated in Table 1.4. As Skehan (2002: 89) explains, “It is important to note here that we are not taking existing aptitude tests and then seeing if SLA relevance can be perceived for each of them. Rather, we are taking SLA stages, and exploring whether aptitude would be relevant for each of these stages”. While some stages in Table 1.4. clear-ly relate to the abilities measured by certain subtests found in the MLAT or the PLAB, in other cases, the model proposed by Skehan “(…) reveals where it would be useful to pro-duce aptitude tests if we are to be able to predict effectively in acquisition-rich contexts” (2002: 90).

Table 1.4. SLA processing stages and potential aptitude components (from Skehan 2002).

SLA Processing stage Aptitude Component

1. noticing auditory segmentation

attention management working memory phonemic coding

2. pattern identification fast analysis/working memory

grammatical sensitivity

3. extending inductive language learning ability

4. complexifying grammatical sensitivity

inductive language learning ability

5. integrating restructuring capacity

6. becoming accurate, avoiding error automatisation

proceduralisation

7. creating a repertoire, achieving salience retrieval processes

8. automatizing rule-based language, achieving fluency automatizing, proceduralisation

(31)

Most recently, Robinson has proposed new models of the concept (Robinson 2002a; Robinson 2002b; Robinson 2007; Robinson 2012; Robinson 2013) and focused on the question whether there are any optimal combinations of ID variables that are conducive to efficient learning. As Robinson (2007: 269) highlights, “The issue then is how best to de-scribe the ID factors and their combinations in such a way as to define sets of aptitudes or optimally conducive sets of abilities for learning (…) during exposure and practice under one condition, or on one task, or accompanied by one FonF technique versus another”.

Fig. 1.1. A hierarchical model of aptitude complexes, ability factors and cognitive abilities (from Robin-son 2007).

The two closely related hypotheses proposed by Robinson are The Aptitude Com-plex Hypothesis and The Ability Differentiation Hypothesis. Robinson (2002b) distguished three conditions of exposure to input: explicit learning (something that is done in-tentionally and requires effort and concentration), implicit learning (knowledge is acquired

(32)

independently of conscious attempts, compared to children’s learning the patterns of the L1) and incidental learning (unintentional learning, compared to children’s L1 implicit learning). The Aptitude Complex Hypothesis, is based on the work of Snow (1994), and claims that “(…) certain sets or combinations of cognitive abilities are drawn on in learning under one condition of instructional L2 exposure versus another” (Robinson 2007: 274). As maintained by Robinson (2012: 67), “Not all learners can be expected to have equivalent aptitudes for learning from each of these options”; therefore “(…) if the effects of instruc-tion and practice are to be optimized for individual learners, then these should take place under those conditions to which their aptitudes are best matched”. Figure 1.1 presents a model of aptitude complexes, ability factors, and cognitive abilities. Aptitude complex 1, for learning from recasting, is a combination of abilities for noticing the gap (NTG) be-tween the recast and the learner’s prior utterance, and memory for contingent speech (MCS). It is argued that these two abilities are crucial for holding the interlocutor’s recast in memory. Aptitude complex 2, for incidental learning from oral input, containing a flood of particular forms, is composed of the ability factor called memory for contingent speech (MCP) and deep semantic processing (DSP). Aptitude complex 3, for incidental learning from frequent exposure to a particular form provided in written input is similar to aptitude complex 2 because it also contains DSP; however, the second component is memory for contingent text (MCT) rather than speech. Finally, aptitude complex 4, for learning from a brief rule explanation, comprises MCT and metalinguistic rule rehearsal (MRR). It is pro-posed that each of the ability factors is measured by different existing aptitude subtests; for example MRR can be measured by the MLAT Words in Sentences (grammatical sensitivi-ty) and Paired Associates (rote memory).

The second part of Robinson’s framework, The Ability Differentiation Hypothesis, was based (among others) on the works of Sparks and Ganschow (1993) and Grigorenko (2002), and on the assumption that some learners have L1-based disabilities which underlie poor aptitude for L2 learning. The theory proposed by Sparks and Ganschow (Sparks and Ganschow 1993; Ganschow et al. 1998; Sparks et al. 2008) was labelled the Linguistic Coding Differences Hypothesis (LCDH). It was the outcome of a line of enquiry on the etiology of foreign language learning difficulties, the main focus of which was on cogni-tive, affective and linguistic domains. The researchers claim that a learner’s level of lan-guage skill and aptitude for learning should be taken into consideration when examining the role of affect in foreign language learning. The LCDH posits that “(…) skills in the native language componentsphonological/orthographic, syntactic, and semanticprovide the

(33)

basic foundation for FL learning” (Ganschow et al. 1998: 248). Robinson (2007: 278) summarized the Ability Differentiation Theory in the following way:

(…) the Ability Differentiation Hypothesis claims that some L2 learners may have more clearly differentiated abilities  and strengths in corresponding aptitude complexes  than other learners and further that it is particularly important to match these learners to conditions of practice which favor their strengths. This is in contrast with other learners who may have less differentiated abilities and equivalent strengths and aptitudes for learning under a variety of conditions of exposure and classroom practice.

As Sáfár and Kormos (2008: 117) claim, “The significance of Robinson’s research is that he investigates the aptitude-treatment interaction, conceiving of language aptitude as a dy-namic construct”.

New conceptualizations of foreign language aptitude required the development of new aptitude tests. For instance, Grigorenko et al. (2000) devised the Cognitive Ability for Novelty in Acquisition of Language as applied to foreign language test (CANAL-FT), which is grounded in cognitive theory, dynamic (test takers are tested at the time of learn-ing) and simulation-based. The primary purpose of the CANAL-FT was to test the CA-NAL-F theory, which holds that “(…) one of the central abilities required for FL acquisi-tion is the ability to cope with novelty and ambiguity” (Grigorenko et al. 2000: 392). The CANAL-FT reflects Sternberg’s Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence, described in the current chapter, section 1.2.1.1. (Sternberg 1985; Sternberg 1996; Sternberg 2002a; Stern-berg 2002b). There are five knowledge acquisition processes underlying the CANAL-FT, which are as follows:

1. Selective encodingdistinguishing between more and less relevant information. 2. Accidental encodingencoding background or secondary information and grasping

the background context of the information stream.

3. Selective comparisondetermining the relevance of old information for current tasks to enhance learning.

4. Selective transferapplying decoded or inferred rules to new contexts and tasks. 5. Selective combinationsynthesizing various pieces of information that have been

collected via selective and accidental encoding.

The five knowledge acquisition processes apply at four levels: lexical, morphologi-cal, semantic, and syntactic and two modes of input and output: (1) visual, predominating in reading and writing and (2) aural, being involved in listening and speaking. As Grigo-renko et al. (2000) argue, for language learning to take place one needs to understand and

(34)

encode the linguistic material into working memory, and then transfer the material to long-term memory for later retrieval. Two types of recall tasks can assess these aspects of encod-ing, storage and retrieval, i.e. immediate recall (right after learning takes place) and delayed recall (at some time interval after learning takes place). The CANAL-FT is composed of nine sections: five of them involve immediate recall and four of them (identical to the five sections) entail delayed recall. These sections are as follows: learning meanings of neolo-gisms from context, understanding the meaning of passages, continuous paired-associate learning, sentential inference and learning language rules. The CANAL-FT focuses on the learning of an artificial language, Ursulu, the rules of which are based on different existing languages, not resembling any language in particular at the same time.

Other aptitude tests summarized by different authors (Grigorenko et al. 2000; Dö-rnyei 2005a; Robinson 2012; DöDö-rnyei and Skehan 2003; Wen and Skehan 2011; Robinson 2005; Parry and Stansfield 1990; Parry and Child 1990; Leaver et al. 2005; Lett and O’Mara 1990) are: DLABthe Defense Language Aptitude Battery (Petersen and Al-Haik 1976), VORD (Parry and Child 1990), York Language Aptitude Test (Green 1975) or the German Aptitude Test (Miller and Philips 1982). However, as Dörnyei (2005a: 41) points out, “there is a general agreement in the literature that the new batteries did not demonstrate superiority over the MLAT”.

The connection between foreign language aptitude and L2 success has been found in a number of different studies. DeKeyser (2000), for example, discovered that aptitude scores are a crucial predictor of achievement in acquisition-rich contexts. Parry and Child (1990) pointed out that foreign language aptitude measured by the MLAT predicts lan-guage learning success more efficiently than the VORD test. Carroll (1981) reported that the correlations between foreign language aptitude and proficiency were between .40 and .60. Other studies also confirmed that aptitude test scores predict success in L2 learning (Carroll 1965; Skehan 1998). Finally, Dörnyei and Skehan (2003: 589) conclude, “(…) individual differences in second language learning, principally foreign language aptitude and motivation, have generated the most consistent predictors of second language learning success. Correlations of aptitude or motivation with language achievement range (mostly) between 0.20 and 0.60, with a median value a little above 0.40”.

An important achievement in foreign language aptitude research in Poland is re-flected in the work of Rysiewicz (2007, 2008b), who made an attempt to investigate the relationship between foreign language aptitude, intelligence and FL achievement scores among seventh grade learners. He used an English achievement test, the Lexicon (Leksykon,

Cytaty

Powiązane dokumenty

It is because of the reasons discussed above that the author of the present paper made a decision to adapt for the Polish FL adult learners the most widely used and known

On the other hand, this success is also evi- dent to the readers, not only because of the value of the papers that appear in the journal, but also the fact that two special issues

rozpoczęto trw ające do 1954 r, system atyczne badania archeologiczne, efektem których było odkrycie naw arstw ień kulturow ych związanych z okresem halsztackim ,

Między rezygnacją a wyzwaniem, czyli ja k pom óc sobie i swojemu umysło­ wo upośledzonemu dziecku.. Zagrożenia i wyzwania

N iniejsze opracow anie przebiegać będzie po linii trzech czło­ nów tem atycznych.. To jest sensem jego życia.. Staw ia więc człow ieka wobec nowego zjaw iska: m

Był podręcznik literatury współczesnej, pióra Wilhelma Feldmana, publicy­ sty, wielokrotnie wydawany, który literaturę „współczesną” zaczynał od Norwi­ da, ale

Po ukazaniu się w Rosji szeregu regionalnych słowników frazeologizmów, przysłów i porze­ kadeł, najnowszych materiałów etnograficznych (Словарь

Język polski jako obcy w szkołach ukraińskich Socjokultura Treści przewidziane w Podstawie Treści spoza Podstawy Powitania pracowników szkoły, sąsiadki, pożegnanie dziadka,