Driver Behavior at Urban Roads in China
1Focus Group Findings
23
Li Jie
4aDepartment of Traffic and Road Engineering, Civil Engineering College, Hunan University, 5
Lushan South Road, 410082 Changsha, Hunan Province, PR China
6 Tel.:+86 13017312752; fax:+86 731 88822667. 7 email: [email protected] 8 9 10
Henk J. van Zuylen
11Department of Transport & Planning, Faculty of Civil Engineering and Geosciences, Delft
12
University of Technology, Stevinweg 1, 2628 CN Delft, The Netherlands,
13 Tel.: +31 626158161 14 e-mail: [email protected] 15 16
Elisabeth van der Horst
17Department of Transport & Planning, Faculty of Civil Engineering and Geosciences, Delft University of
18
Technology, Stevinweg 1, 2628 CN Delft, The Netherlands,
19
e-mail: [email protected]
20 21 22
Submitted for presentation to the 93th Annual Meeting of the Transportation Research Board 23
January 12 – 16, 2014 24
25
Word count:6592words + 2 Tables *250 + 2 Figures * 250 = 7592 equivalent words 26 27 Paper 14- 4617 28 29 30 31
Revised paper, submitted November 14, 2013 32
ABSTRACT 1
Driver behavior in China shows remarkable differences from that in western countries. In this study, six focus 2
groups were organized to investigate Chinese drivers’ attitudes, expectations, intended actions, their preferences, 3
and habits in different situations in urban areas. The outcomes show that there are important opportunities to 4
improve driver behavior, leading to safer and more efficient traffic. 5
Driver behavior–related simulation models have been developed for traffic plan, evaluation and design purpose. 6
Most of these models consider relatively ideal driver behavior in western countries in the – for them common - 7
traffic conditions. This study shows what modifications and calibration are needed to make such models valid for 8
Chinese traffic. 9
10
Keywords: Focus group; Driver behavior; Traffic Offences; Traffic Priority Rules; China
11 12 13
INTRODUCTION 1
In the past decades, various algorithms have been developed to model driver behavior in given traffic conditions. 2
Driver behavior, strongly related to the traffic capacity, safety and variability, is an important component of 3
simulation models. These models usually have many parameters to represent drivers’ characteristics such as: 4
desired acceleration/ deceleration, critical gap, safe distance, desired speed and so on. However, there is not 5
much literature related to the influence of local traffic conditions on driver behavior and the improvement of 6
existing models based on the driving custom. 7
China became the second biggest vehicle country and the largest vehicle market in the world from 2011. The 8
vehicle ownership and the number of drivers both increased in the past decade (1).The driving custom and 9
behavior in China differ from these in western countries, which spent more than 100 years to achieve the present 10
road traffic development level. Field observations have shown that the saturation flow rates at Chinese signalized 11
intersections are about 20% to 30% lower than the values at similar intersections in The Netherlands (2). Deeper 12
insight into the driver behavior in China is expected not only to offer significant information to adjust the driver 13
behavior models, but also find effective measures to improve traffic performance in China. 14
Investigations on driver behavior are usually based on external observations, such as vehicle trajectories, 15
video observation, and surveys, which all cannot provide information with regard to drivers’ attitudes and 16
opinions. Focus groups can shed some more light on these issues. Another stimulus to organize focus group 17
discussions on driver behavior in China came from a survey with questionnaires executed in 2013 in China and 18
The Netherlands at the same time (1). The comparison shows remarkable differences in driver behavior in these 19
two countries. The research questions for this study are which factors have a large influence on driver behavior 20
in China and how driver behavior can be improved. 21
Focus group discussions are an appropriate method to collect relevant information. Focus groups go back to 22
the 1930s when social scientists wanted to move from close-ended interviews to open discussions. Focus groups 23
became increasingly popular because they can assist in understanding how people feel or think about an issue. 24
Focus groups create a permissive environment that helps participants to feel comfortable and disclose themselves 25
(3). 26
In this study, focus groups were used to 27
• Get insight into the factors influencing driver behavior in urban traffic condition, 28
• Understand changes of driver behavior in different traffic conditions from the drivers’ perspective, 29
• Investigate drivers’ perceptions concerning vehicle interactions, traffic rules and especially priority 30
rules. 31
The results can give directions to adjust existing driving models implemented in micro simulators or programs. 32
Lastly, results can provide information on how to improve driver behavior, traffic management and traffic 33
performance in urban areas. 34
The following section summarizes literature related to existing driving models, driver behavior investigations, 35
and focus group research related to driver behavior. Next, a description of the focus group experiment is 36
presented. The following section summarizes participants’ discussion on the driver behavior on five different 37
aspects. Finally, the last section gives some conclusions. 38
LITERATURE REVIEW 1
The literature review in this study mainly refers to three topics: driving models, driver behavior investigations, 2
and focus group studies associated with drivers. For every topic, many studies have been carried out in the past. 3
In this section, only the studies strongly related to driver behavior investigation are considered. 4
Studies about driving models have a practical application in micro-simulators, and the analysis of traffic safety 5
and capacity. Most existing models explain driver behavior with variables related both to the personal attributes 6
(e.g. desired speed, desired acceleration and deceleration) and to surrounding vehicles (e.g. relative speed and 7
distance). However, most available models ignore some aspects of human attitudes and actions such as: 8
perceiving stimuli, evaluating alternative control actions, selecting an appropriate control action, and carrying 9
out this action. 10
In the commercial microscopic simulation programs, like CORSIM, VISSIM (4), S-Paramics (5), 11
AIMSUN(6,7), and FOSIM (8) the movements of individual vehicles can be simulated with different parameter 12
settings. For example, in FOSIM, five different types of driver/vehicle combinations are distinguished with 13
different parameter values. In S-Paramics, besides the different types of vehicles, different driving types are 14
characterized by aggression and awareness factors. Similarly CORSIM can distinguish 10 different kinds of 15
drivers. In VISSIM two different specifications of the Wiedemann (8) car-following model can be selected. In a 16
simulation model, all drivers drive according to the same car following model but differences may exist in the 17
parameter values for each group of drivers to represent their specific, e.g. aggressive or non-aggressive driving 18
(10). 19
There are several frequently used methods for the investigation of driver behavior. Driver behavior observed 20
on the road can be considered as unfeigned, but is usually limited to the short time period of data gathering (e.g. 21
video observation, trajectory analysis) and a limited number of drivers (e.g. probe vehicle). A simple way of 22
measuring driver behavior is to have a Driver Behavior Questionnaire (DBQ) survey on the drivers what are 23
their typical behavior attitudes in driving ( 11,12,13,14,15). But these investigation methods can’t provide too 24
much information with respect to drivers’ opinion and thinking process. 25
Focus group discussion is a practical way to obtain qualitative information about drivers’ perception. At the 26
beginning, focus group discussion was often used by social scientists, and then became popular in driver 27
behavior study in the past decade. In Hong Kong, focus groups were used to investigate the attitudes, beliefs, and 28
perceptions of the public towards red light cameras (16). Focus groups were also conducted to investigate 29
commuter drivers’ perceptions of freeway level of service (17, 18). Sun, Kondyli et al. studied lane changing 30
behavior and ramp merging behavior on freeways through focus group discussions, and furthermore the 31
participating drivers were categorized according to their background information and verbal responses (19, 20). 32
From the view of theory, the understanding about driver behavior is critical to get valid simulation models. 33
Focus groups can assist in understanding drivers’ attitudes and opinions about driver behavior, identifying what 34
factors affect drivers’ decisions, consequently help people to find effective ways to improve driver behavior and 35
traffic performance. 36
FOCUS GROUP EXPERIMENT DESIGN 1
This section describes the organization of the focus groups and presents the questions discussed during the 2
sessions 3
Setting up Focus Groups
4
The focus group study was organized in Changsha, a city of 7 million inhabitants in the middle of China. The 5
participants were selected from respondents of the previous questionnaire study varying with respect to gender, 6
age, driving experience and profession (21). In this questionnaire study, every candidate was required to 7
complete a questionnaire which is explicitly described in (21) . The questionnaire assembled information both on 8
socio-demographic characteristics and driver behavior. Totally, 35 participants were invited to join six 2-hour 9
focus groups respectively. In each session, participants were also required to complete two survey forms 10
associated with lane changing and overtaking behavior. 11
Participants Socio-demographic characteristics
12
Totally 6 focus groups involving 35 participants (28 males and 7 females; range 25–50 years) are organized in 13
this study. The largest proportion of vehicles driven by participants was reported to be their own vehicle (71.4%), 14
and a small proportion was from a company or institute (22.9%) or others (5.7%), etc. The participants had 15
different education background: Professional education 10 (28.6%), Bachelor 11 (31.4%), Master or higher than 16
Master 6 (17.1%), and Others 8 (22.9%). Most of participants (25.7%) were Professional Persons; 17
Administrative staff and Professional driver both constitute 20%. Most participants, i.e. 60%, stated they enjoy 18
driving. There were only few participants don’t have special feeling or dislike for driving with the percentages of 19
28.6% and 11.4% respectively. 20
21
FOCUS GROUP QUESTIONS 22
Focus group discussions were around three categories questions: opening questions, practical questions and 23
ending questions, which are all presented in this section. The opening question was designed to encourage 24
people to start talking and make them feel comfortable. The practical questions included five application 25
questions: car following, lane changing, overtaking, reaction to traffic signals, opinion about traffic rules and 26
priority giving, which are all relevant to driver behavior models. The central idea of these practical questions was 27
to identify the relevant factors and how these factors could influence driver behavior. The ending question 28
ensured that all important topics have been covered. 29
The data from the focus groups contained (a) voice recordings of the 2-hours sessions, (b) questionnaire about 30
socio-demographic characteristics and driving behavior, and (c) additional investigation tables filled in during 31
the sessions. 32
Opening Questions
33
Q1: How do you think about Chinese people’s driving behavior? Why? 34
Most of participants thought Chinese drivers’ behavior is not so well in terms of the offenses and aberrant 35
driving behavior, like sudden or forced lane changing without indication light, no knowledge about priority rules 1
in conflict area, etc. They presented several causes resulting in such behavior: 2
1) China entered the motorized vehicle era only about ten years ago, and the related education system, 3
license examination system, and law system are all in a developing phase. The western countries have developed 4
the whole system related to motorized traffic for more than 100 years. There are differences in driving culture 5
and customs between China and more advanced countries. 6
2) In China, about one third of the drivers are novices who have limited knowledge to evaluate the driving 7
environment and conditions compared with experienced drivers. So these novel drivers behave quite 8
heterogeneously: some are quite aggressive and some drive too conservatively. 9
3) The traffic monitoring and management systems in China are also in development. Stricter monitoring 10
can help drivers to develop better driving behavior. If the disobedience isn’t penalized, the consequence is that 11
more drivers continue to infringe the rules. 12
13
Q2. Do you enjoy driving? Why? 14
21 of the 28 male participants denoted they enjoyed driving quite a lot with different reasons, for instance 15
convenience, feeling of freedom, pleasure of sports, and the quiet private space. All 7 female participants 16
expressed they dislike or don’t have special feeling with driving. The causes which lead to dislike driving are 17
congestion, difficulty in parking, risk of accidents, etc. It can be concluded from these focus group discussions 18
that most male drivers like driving; and most female drivers are the revers. 19
20
Q3. Is your driving style conservative or aggressive? What factor can influence your driving style? 21
Even most participants thought Chinese drivers’ driving behavior is not good, only 5 of the 35 participants 22
admitted they are somewhat aggressive in driving and all the other participants considered themselves as a 23
conservative or common driver. Participants explained that their driving style can be influenced by lots of 24
different factors. 25
The mostly mentioned factors influencing driver behavior are traffic conditions, weather, driving task, and 26
personal mood. In hurry and congested situations, the driving style can become more aggressive than in a normal 27
situation. Most drivers admitted their driving style is strongly related to the local traffic rule enforcement system. 28
Based on the discussions, the influence of personal mood on driving style can vary among different drivers. 29
Some participants expressed a cheerful mood can make them more patient in driving. But there are also some 30
participants who said they easily make some driving errors in a cheerful status. Some participants stated they 31
become more aggressive when they are tired because they want to arrive at the destination as soon as possible; 32
other participants expressed they would be more careful in driving to avoid traffic incidents when they are tired. 33
So there is a large variation in the reaction of drivers from external and personal issues 34
35
Application 1: Car following behavior
36
In the focus group discussion, car following was explained as driving behind another vehicle on a short distance. 37
Q1. What factors do you think are most important in “car following”? 1
Most of participants indicated ‘safe distance to the preceding vehicle’ is very important in car following situation. 2
The identification of the ‘safe distance’ varies not only among different drivers, but also between different traffic 3
situations. In order to prevent from forced lane changing in congested traffic condition, participants stated they 4
prefer to keep a quite short distance with the preceding vehicle. But in an uncongested situation and driving at 5
high speed, they fear a sudden lane changing in front of them made by an adjacent driver or a sudden braking of 6
the preceding vehicle, so they are inclined to keep a larger headway, because the aberrant behavior at high speed 7
can result in quite serious traffic accidents. These descriptions are consistent with the factual traffic phenomena: 8
drivers keep quite short space headways in a bottleneck; but after passing the bottleneck, the drivers seem to 9
relax and keep quite long headways. 10
In car following status, the experienced drivers stated they often try to keep a long distance from the 11
preceding vehicle, because they want a better view so that they can make proper decisions in time. In order to 12
have a wide view, they often drive along the marking line rather than in the middle of the lane. Compared with 13
experienced drivers, novices concentrate mainly on the preceding vehicle and pay less attention to the whole 14
traffic surroundings. 15
Experienced drivers described that they do not dare to accelerate quickly in dense traffic on up-hill road 16
sections, because there is potential risk of rear collisions if the vehicle in front suddenly brakes and they still 17
have their foot on the gas pedal. On a down-hill section, it is safe even driving at a high speed because drivers 18
usually don’t have their foot on the gas pedal and can quickly brake. 19
20
Application 2: Lane Changing behavior
21
Q1. What factors do you think are most important in “lane changing”? Which kind of lane changing 22
habits do you dislike? Do you accelerate or decelerate while making lane changing? 23
Most participants stated that the available safe gap is most important in lane changing. 24
Participants listed the most frequent aberrant lane changing behavior: no indication light for lane changing, 25
forced lane changing, and long-time driving on the marking between lanes. 26
Quite significant difference exists in lane changing behavior between male and female participants with 27
respect to speed. Most male participants told that they always accelerate during lane changing because they want 28
to finish the lane changing procedure as soon as possible. On the contrary, most female participants stated they 29
are inclined to decelerate in lane changing with the reason they can easily stop lane changing at any moment if 30
necessary. Also some participants stated that they often accelerate when changing lanes in free flow, and 31
decelerate in dense flow. They explained the reason: in free flow a quick change of lanes is clear to drivers 32
behind them and these drivers can react in good time if necessary; in dense traffic lane changing has to be done 33
slowly because the gap to merge into has to be forced by pushing his car slowly into the other lane and more 34
cooperation with the following vehicle is required. 35
36
Q2. Cooperation and competition in lane changing 37
The interactions between vehicles involved in lane changing can be described as either cooperation (the follower 38
courteously yields to create a gap and allows the leader to move to the front of him/her) or competition (the 1
follower doesn’t acknowledge the intention of the leader, but the leader still try to make a gap for lane changing) 2
(20). 3
In the focus group sessions, the participants are required to describe their actions in two scenarios. 4
Scenario 1. You need to change lane 5
Do your actions differ between the following two lane changing situations: changing to the right lane or
6
changing to the left? Does any difference exist between normal and congested traffic situations?
7
Participants explained that lane changing to the most right lane was more difficult and required a longer gap 8
than to the most left lane. One reason is based on the fact: the blind spot on the right side of the vehicle is larger 9
than left side one (for a vehicle with the steer at left). The other reason is that the right lane generally has more 10
disturbances, e.g. holes in the pavement, pedestrians, mopeds and electric bicycles. In conclusion, the critical gap 11
for lane changing to the right side should be much larger than to the left side. 12
In urban areas, the difference in lane changing between different traffic situations is represented in the location 13
where a lane changing decision is made. In dense flow, participants are concerned for less chance to change lane 14
and start lane changing much earlier than in free flow conditions. 15
16
Scenario 2. The other vehicle is changing lane, but there isn’t a sufficiently large gap in front of you. 17
Do your react differently between the following two lane changing scenarios:
18
(a) You are in the most right lane and the other vehicle in the left lane is attempting to merge in front of you;
19
(b) You are in the median lane and the other vehicle is planning to merge in front of you.
20
For the first scenario, most participants showed they are willing to yield to create an available gap for a forced 21
lane changing when they are in the most right lane because they think the driver changing lane to the right 22
possibly wants to turn to the curb-side lane and exit this street. But they are not willing to create a gap to a forced 23
lane changing when they are in the median lane because the other vehicle disturbs them to a certain extent. Few 24
participants (6/35) denoted they don’t cooperate in any kind of forced lane changing. 25
26
Q3. Likelihood of a lane changing 27
In order to identify the importance of the potential factors to lane changing, participants were asked to evaluate 28
the level of likelihood (from 1 to 5) of initiating a lane changing for each listed scenario according to their 29
driving experience. For every given scenario, there are two different driving situations: commuting and leisure. 30
The assumption of traffic environment is urban street with three lanes for each direction. The results are shown 31
in Table 1. 32
TABLE 1 Likelihood of making a lane changing (1 = generally would not, 5 = generally would) Scenario 1.1.1. Commuting 1.1.2. Leisure Average Importance (Max=5) Importance Std Average Importance (Max=5) Importance Std
Stopped bus at a bus stop 3.8 1.3 3.1 1.4
Vehicle merging into your lane 2.5 1.5 2.0 1.2
Slow-moving vehicle ahead 4.0 1.2 3.7 1.2
Shorter line of queuing vehicles in another lane
3.6 1.2 3.2 1.2
Bus or truck in front of you 3.7 1.2 3.5 1.5
Vehicle tailgating you 2.0 1.2 1.8 1.2
Undesirable pavement conditions 3.6 1.4 3.3 1.4
1
The front slow-moving vehicle is the most important reason for a lane change. In commuting, stopped bus 2
and preceding vehicle type (bus or truck) are the other two factors with quite high likelihood to stimulate a lane 3
changing. Vehicle merging and vehicle tailgating both has a low likelihood of lane changing. The likelihood of 4
lane changing in a leisure trip is much lower than in the trip for work in every given scenario, which proves 5
again the driving behavior can vary to certain extent in terms of travel targets. 6
7
Application 3: Overtaking behavior
8
In the focus group discussion, overtaking is defined as passing another vehicle. Several topics are discussed in 9
the focus group sessions. 10
11
Q1. What factors do you think are most important during “overtaking”? What kind of overtaking 12
habit do you dislike? 13
Participants agreed that speed and wide view of the whole traffic surrounding are critical for overtaking. Some 14
experienced drivers emphasized sufficient preparation work should be made to inform the preceding vehicle that 15
it will be overtaken. They often signal with their headlights and the horn, even though these seem aggressive 16
from western drivers’ view. 17
Participants also demonstrated several aberrant overtaking behavior, for instance still executing overtaking 18
when the vehicle in front is also overtaking, and overtaking in spite of an insufficient gap. 19
Overtaking at the right side is quite common in urban areas in China. The participants gave several reasons: 20
1) There are often more lanes at urban streets than at a freeway, so forbidden overtaking at the right side is not 21
a strict rule in urban areas; 22
2) Lots of drivers prefer to drive on the most left lane, which leads to no chance to overtake via the left lane; 23
3) The most left lane has fewer disturbances and better pavement than the most right lane, which both 1
stimulate drivers to drive more often on the most left lane. 2
3
Q2. Cooperation and competition in overtaking 4
The participants were asked to describe their actions when they overtake on a dual carriage way or two way 5
road. They explained that they follow the standard procedure (look for a gap, turn on the indicator, accelerate 6
and change lane). Most of participants don’t think it is important to change back to the original lane in urban 7
areas. 8
Participants also explained that they were not told that they should not stay on most left lane for a long time on 9
urban roads in the driving lessons. These reasons all lead to the phenomenon that drivers use the most left lanes 10
more often than the most right lane in China. 11
12
Application 4: Reaction to signal
13
Traffic management and driver behavior at intersections are critical for the performance of the whole network in 14
urban areas. In the focus group sessions, drivers’ reaction to traffic lights is discussed through several key 15
questions. 16
17
Q1. What are your reactions when you see the light turns to green and you are the first driver in the 18
queue? 19
Most of participants expressed they will first release the brake, then check if there are still pedestrians in the 20
conflict area, and finally accelerate. 21
Experienced drivers differ with novice drivers in the reaction to green light on the fact that they care about 22
their vision at the beginning of a green phase. If their vision is obstructed by another vehicle, they do not dare to 23
accelerate quickly because there are possibly some conflicting vehicles or pedestrians hidden. Most participants 24
told that they are very careful in the beginning of green phase and do not dare to accelerate directly and quickly 25
due to possible conflicting traffic flows. 26
27
Q2. What are your actions when you see the signal turning to yellow? 28
Many participants were not clear about the appropriate actions during the yellow phase. To this question, most 29
participants expressed they should decelerate and stop; 9 participants stated they always speed up, 21 30
participants said they always decelerate, and only 5 participants indicated that their action depends on the speed 31
and distance to the stop line. 32
Participants stated they never got any special instruction concerning the reaction to yellow phase in their 33
driving license course and the coach often emphasizes to drive carefully and brake in any urgent case in the 34
intersection area. Most participants agreed that crossing stop line during yellow phase is a potential cause for 35
traffic accidents. 36
Q4. What factors can influence your reaction to traffic lights? Are you aware of some mistakes in your 1
reaction to the signal? 2
Most participants presented the location of the signal can influence their reaction to signal to a certain extent. In 3
China, signals are often installed on the far side of the intersection, rather than above or beside the stop line. 4
Drivers’ view can be easily blocked by a bus, even by a SUV in dense traffic conditions. At some intersections, 5
it is difficult to find the stop line, which can also influence the reaction to signals. 6
Many of participants admitted that, waiting for a signal, their attention can be easily distracted by some private 7
activity, like their mobile phone, chatting with other passengers, etc. Five participants stated they had the 8
experience to cross the stop line during the red phase because they only focused on the preceding vehicle and 9
ignored the signal transition. 10
11
Q5. What is your attitude to a conflicting vehicle that is still remaining at the intersection at the beginning 12
of green? 13
There were only 14 out of the 35 participants told that they always give priority to these conflicting vehicles in 14
such scenario by driving slowly or around such vehicles, very few participants stated they will fully stop. 15
Eighteen participants explained they will not give priority in case of other drivers behind them push them or if 16
they are in a hurry. Three participants stated they never gave priority to the remaining conflicting vehicles. 17
To sum up, participants’ attitude to the remaining conflict vehicles at a signalized intersection is diverse and 18
the priority rule in this situation is quite unclear to most drivers. 19
20
Application 5: opinion of traffic rules
21
Traffic rules, as an important component of traffic management, are not only critical to traffic safety, but also 22
relevant to simulation models. Most simulation models comprise the official traffic rules, but in reality drivers 23
disregard these rules in some cases. Several questions associated with traffic rules, especial with priority rules 24
were discussed in the focus group sessions. 25
26
Q1. Tell some rules related to priority. Are these rules important? Why? 27
Most participants only expressed two priority rules: turning vehicles should always give priority to through 28
going vehicles and vehicles should always give priority to pedestrians on a zebra. Only four participants knew 29
that drivers should give priority to the vehicles coming from the right side. 30
The participants were required to rank the priority in three scenarios as shown as follows: 31
(a) (b) (c) 1
2
FIGURE 1 Conflict traffic flow in three scenarios 3
In China, the priority rules for un-signalized intersection (without priority sign or traffic police directing in 4
field) are listed as follows: 5
1. Left side flow gives priority to right side flow; 6
2. Turning flow gives priority to through going flow; 7
3. Right turning gives priority to left turning vehicles if they are from the opposite direction. 8
When the condition can match more than one rule, the later rule should be implemented. For example, 9
scenario (a) measures up both rule 1 and rule 2, the rule 2 should be enforced, i.e. vehicle 2 should give priority 10
to vehicle 1. 11
But in most western countries, like The Netherlands – which was used as a comparison -, the priority rules for 12
the same un-signalized intersection are simple, as described as follows: 13
1. Left side flow gives priority to right side flow; 14
2. Turning flow gives priority to the opposite through going flow. 15
In terms of these two different priority rule settings, the priority can be ranked as: 16
• Scenario (a): 1>3>2 (in European countries the sequence is 3>2>1>3, with courtliness in case this rule 17
leads for grid-lock) 18
• Scenario (b): 1>2>3 (same in Europe) 19
• Scenario (c): 2>3>1 (in Europe 1>2>3) 20
There was only one participant who gave the correct answers for all three scenarios in terms of the Chinese 21
priority rules. All the other 34 participants were not clear about the priority rules and even the experienced 22
drivers said they never learned these priority rules. What was mentioned mostly in conflict situations in reality is 23
‘courtesy’, not only emphasized by driving coaches, but also by traffic police and newspaper, etc. Through the 24
focus group discussion, all participants agreed on the importance of such priority rules, and they also found that 25
the rules in western countries are much easier to be applied in practice. 26
27
Q2. Do you know some priority traffic signs? Do you see them in reality? 28
All participants were asked to write down the meaning of several traffic signs related to priority rules. They are 29
signs to give priority by an obligatory full stop or by deceleration; they are on the road pavement or on the road 30
side respectively, as shown in Figure 2: 31
1
(a) (b) (c) (d) 2
FIGURE 2 Priority sign: give priority by an obligatory stop (a, c), and by deceleration (b, d) 3
The answers made by the 35 participants are rather peculiar: only 2 persons from the 35 participants could 4
give the correct answers to the four priority signs. More details about the answers are shown in Table 2. Most 5
participants gave wrong answers, or admitted they didn’t know or had never seen them. A few participants only 6
gave ambiguous answers, like deceleration, carefully driving. 22 participants even didn’t believe there is a traffic 7
rule to give priority by a fully stop. 8
TABLE 2 Answers to recognizing priority sign 9
Location Sign Correct Answer
Ambiguous Answer
False Answer
Unknown Never Seen
Road side sign Give priority by Stop 4 0 9 12 10 Give priority by deceleration 3 3 1 14 14 On the road pavement Give priority by Stop 5 1 11 17 1 Give priority by deceleration 4 14 1 1.1.3. 15 1.1.4. 1 10
The participants were shown several other common traffic signs such as: forbidden stopping, forbidden 11
parking, one-way street, and forbidden driving respectively. Except the ‘forbidden parking’ sign, most of the 12
participants did not know the other signs clearly. Another rather peculiar finding is that the word 禁停- ‘Jin 13
Ting’ -can mean both ‘forbidden stopping’ and ‘forbidden parking’ in Chinese. The double meaning of the sign 14
‘禁停’ leads to confusion in understanding, namely people who know the name of the sign, but maybe do not 15
know the meaning correctly. After such a small test in focus group discussion, most participants admitted they 16
didn’t have the habit to search for traffic signs, especially signs on the road pavement. Participants expressed 17
they don’t reinforce their memory about some of the driving rules and traffic signs after getting the driving 18
license. 19
20
Q3. Do you think some traffic rules are important and others less important? Why? 21
Most participants stated traffic rules are important, especially the traffic rules related to traffic signals. In 20 22
participant’s eyes, some rules are irrational, for example too low speed limitation (e.g. 40km/h) on national road 23
outside an urban area, too long solid lines between lanes to forbid lane changing. If without strict enforcement, 1
these participants told they will ignore these irrational rules. 2
3
Q4. What offenses do you often make? 4
In the focus group sessions, the most often mentioned offenses are illegal parking, crossing solid line to change 5
lane, and speeding on freeways or national roads. Several participants also declared driving over the wrong lane 6
sometimes due to the inappropriately designed signal control. For example, too long queue on the left-turning 7
lane due to too short green time for left turning will make them lose patience and take the through going lane to 8
make a turn left. 9
Most experienced professional drivers told they were already quite familiar with the local traffic monitoring 10
system and very few offences were recorded in the past year. According to the questionnaire, the average 11
number of offences in the past year is 1.4 times for the professional drivers, and 1.8 times for the other 12
participants. Every participant expressed that traffic monitoring and enforcement of the rules will help drivers to 13
improve their driver behavior. 14
Ending Questions
15
The ending question is ‘How to improve driver behavior in China’. The suggestions are concluded as follows: 16
• Being stricter with driving license examination. More attention should be given to the traffic rules and 17
opinions of driving safety, rather than the driving skills. In the driving license examination, more driving tests 18
on the road with real traffic surroundings should be executed. 19
• Offering more education about driver behavior and traffic rules through radio broadcast, TV program, 20
Newspaper etc. For example: traffic police enforcing traffic rules on the road by analyzing the traffic accidents 21
cause, traffic rules and responsibility, etc. These measures are expected to increase the knowledge of traffic 22
rules for all of citizens. 23
• Introducing traffic education system into schools as what have done in western countries for decades, e.g. 24
teaching children at the age of seven the traffic rules for pedestrian and giving a cycling examination at the 25
age of 10. 26
CONCLUSION AND DISCUSSION 27
Through the focus group discussions, several specific issues related to Chinese drivers’ attitudes, driving 28
manners and opinions became clear. The driving culture in China is less sophisticated than this in Western 29
countries mainly due to a shorter history of the motorized traffic. The fraction of novice drivers with limited 30
experience is rather large in China. Drivers’ knowledge about traffic rules which they have learned in driver 31
license courses is not reinforced by the daily driving experience, but fades away. Priority rules are not known by 32
most drivers who have to behave carefully because they are not certain about what other drivers will do. In such 33
traffic culture, conflicts can’t be solved according to general traffic rules and the traffic efficiency becomes low 34
in practice. This finding gives an explanation for the conclusion that the saturation flows in Chinese cities are 35
considerably lower than in comparable situation in a Western country, drawn in the previous research (2). 36
The Chinese participants told that the driving conditions (like congestion, long delays) have an important 1
influence on the way they take decisions. This is partly supported by the finding in (2) that saturation flows 2
depend on the size of intersections and on traffic volumes: larger intersections with more traffic have a higher 3
saturation flow rates. 4
Lane changing and overtaking have some special characteristics. First of all, lane changing to a left lane is 5
different from a movement to a right lane. Courtesy yielding is more likely to be provided for cars moving to the 6
right lane and not common for lane changing to a left lane. Since many drivers continue to drive on the leftmost 7
lane, overtaking has often to be done on a right lane. If it is done via a left lane, the overtaking car often does not 8
move back to the original lane. The use of the rightmost lane is not as common as the leftmost one. Gap 9
acceptance depends on the traffic situation and in heavy traffic it is often done by gap enforcing. 10
The personal condition, such as hurry, irritation and impatience, is an important determinant of the driver 11
behavior. Therefore it is necessary in simulation programs to have the possibility to specify different kinds of 12
drivers, such as aggressive or conservative drivers – even though the same driver can switch between these 13
characteristics. The distribution over conservative and aggressive drivers should depend on the traffic situation. 14
In practice, priority in conflict situation is not always given according to rules, which makes it very difficult 15
to predict how a conflict will be solved. In this case, the delay due to conflicts at intersections and merging 16
sections will in general increase compared with the case that the conflict is solved according to rules. 17
The results of this research show lots of opportunities for improving driver behavior in Chinese cities. This 18
can be realized by stronger enforcement of the traffic rules, improving driving license examination system, and 19
more education of road users. 20
This study also shows that simulation models developed in western countries should be adapted to Chinese 21
driving conditions. Especially saturation flows (2), lane changing and priority giving are considerably different. 22
Also the influence of traffic conditions on driver behavior should be considered. The parameters of the car 23
following and lane changing models should be at least calibrated and be modified according to the local traffic 24
situations. Furthermore, some variations in driving style, namely aggressive level, should be introduced. 25
26 27
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 28
This research has been made possible by grants for Changsha Science and Technology Commission under 29
number K1001010-11 and K1106004-11. Also the financial support of vZC is acknowledged. 30
The conclusions in this paper are the responsibility of the authors. 31
32
REFERENCE 33
1. LI, J. and H. J. van Zuylen (2013). Road Traffic in China. EWGT2013 - 16th Meeting of the EURO 34
Working Group on Transportation, Porto, Portugal. 35
2. LI, J., H. J. van Zuylen, et al. (2011). "Comparison of driver behaviour and saturation flow in China and 36
the Netherlands." IET INTELLIGENT TRANSPORT SYSTEMS 6(3): 10. 37
3. Krueger, R. A. and M. A. Cassey (2000). "Focus Groups: A Practical Guide for Applied Research (3rd 38
ed.). ." Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, Calif. 39
4. Bloomberg, L. and Dale, J. (2000). Comparison of VISSIM and CORSIM Traffic Simulation Models on 1
a Congested Network, Transportation Research Record 1727, 52 - 60 2
5. Modeller V3.0 User Guide and Reference Manual. Quadstone Ltd., Edinburgh, United Kingdom, 2000. 3
6. Ciuffo, B. Punzo, V., Torrieri, V. (2008) Comparison of Simulation-Based and Model-Based 4
Calibrations of Traffic-Flow Microsimulation Models. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the 5
Transportation Research Board, No. 2088, Transportation Research Board of the National Academies, 6
Washington, D.C., 2008, pp. 36–44. 7
7. Barceló, J., J. Ferrer, J. Casas, L. Montero, and J. Perarnau (2002), Microscopic simulation with 8
AIMSUN for the assessment of incident management strategies, e-safety Congress and Exhibition: IT 9
solutions for safety and security in intelligent transport, Lyon. 10
8. Fosim 5.1 (2006). Developer: Delft University of Technology, supported by Enigmatry, Website: 11
http://www.fosim.nl/ 12
9. Wiedemann, R. (1991) Modelling of RTI elements on multi-lane roads. In: Advanced Telematics in 13
Road Transport, Vol. II, pp. 1001-1019, Elsevier, Netherlands. 14
10. Chiabaut, N., L. Leclercq, et al. (2010). "From heterogeneous drivers to macroscopic patterns in 15
congestion." Transportation Research Part B: Methodological 44(2): 299-308. 16
11. Lajunen, T., D. Parker, et al. (2004). "The Manchester Driver Behaviour Questionnaire: a cross-cultural 17
study." Accident Analysis & Prevention 36(2): 231-238. 18
12. Davey, J., D. Wishart, et al. (2007). "An application of the driver behaviour questionnaire in an 19
Australian organisational fleet setting." Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and 20
Behaviour 10(1): 11-21. 21
13. Bener, A., T. Özkan, et al. (2008). "The Driver Behaviour Questionnaire in Arab Gulf countries: Qatar 22
and United Arab Emirates." Accident Analysis & Prevention 40(4): 1411-1417. 23
14. Zhao, N., B. Mehler, et al. (2012). "An investigation of the relationship between the driver behavior 24
questionnaire and objective measures of highway driver behavior." Transportation Research Part F: 25
Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 15(6): 676-685. 26
15. Kircher, K. and J. Andersson (2013). "Truck Drivers’ Opinion on Road Safety in Tanzania—A 27
Questionnaire Study." Traffic Injury Prevention 14(1): 103-111. 28
16. Wissinger, L. M., J. E. Hummer, et al. (2000). Using focus groups to investigate issues of red light 29
running. Highway and Traffic Safety: Engineering, Evaluation, and Enforcement; Trucking and 30
Motorcycles: Safety and Human Performance: 38-45. 31
17. Hall, F., S. Wakefield, et al. (2001). Freeway quality of service - What really matters to drivers and 32
passengers? Traffic Flow Theory and Highway Capacity 2001: Highway Operations, Capacity, and 33
Traffic Control: 17-23. 34
18. Hostovsky, C., S. Wakefield, et al. (2004). Freeway users' perceptions of quality of service - 35
Comparison of three groups. Traffic Flow Theory and Highway Capacity and Quality of Services 2004: 36
150-157. 37
19. Kondyli, A. and L. Elefteriadou (2011). "Modeling Driver Behavior at Freeway-Ramp Merges." 38
Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board 2249(-1): 29-37. 39
20. Sun, D. and L. Elefteriadou (2011). "Information Categorization Based on Driver Behavior for Urban 1
Lane-Changing Maneuvers." Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research 2
Board 2249(-1): 89-94. 3
21. LI, J., H. J. van Zuylen, et al. (2013). The Driver Behaviour Questionnaire: An investigation study 4
applied to Chinese drivers. EWGT2013 - 16th Meeting of the EURO Working Group on 5
Transportation, Porto, Portugal. 6