• Nie Znaleziono Wyników

Badanie pilotażowe Kwestionariusza Zasobów Rozwoju

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Badanie pilotażowe Kwestionariusza Zasobów Rozwoju"

Copied!
22
0
0

Pełen tekst

(1)

Annales Universitatis Paedagogicae Cracoviensis

Studia Psychologica 10 (2017)

ISSN 2084-5596

DOI 10.24917/20845596.10.3

Maciej Pasowicz1

Faculty of Management and Social Communication, Jagiellonian University

Pilot Test of the Growth Resources Questionnaire

Abstract

In this paper, the results of a pilot test of a new diagnostic tool are presented. The Growth Resources Questionnaire (its Polish version) was developed on the basis of the Growth Resources Model – a new theoretical concept dedicated to grasping the key psychosocial resources responsible for personal development and flourishing (Pasowicz, 2017, in this volume). The questionnaire consists of three scales: The Positive Autonomy Scale, The Positive Belonging Scale, and The Positive Emotionality Scale. The questionnaire was tested on a sample of 304 subjects and its most important psychometric properties are presented and discussed. Finally, conclusions are drawn and further developments of the tool are outlined.

Key words: resources, autonomy, belonging, emotions, questionnaire.

Badanie pilotażowe Kwestionariusza Zasobów Rozwoju Streszczenie

W  artykule  zaprezentowane  są  wyniki  pilotażu  nowego  narzędzia  diagnostycznego.  Kwestionariusz  Zasobów  Rozwoju  został  opracowany  na  podstawie  Modelu  Zasobów  Rozwoju – nowej koncepcji teoretycznej opisującej kluczowe psychospołeczne zasoby odpo-wiedzialne za osobisty rozwój oraz rozkwit (Pasowicz, 2017, w tym tomie). Kwestionariusz składa się z trzech skal: Skali Pozytywnej Autonomii, Skali Pozytywnej Przynależności oraz  Skali Pozytywnej Emocjonalności. Narzędzie zostało przetestowane z udziałem grupy 304  osób i przedstawione są jego najważniejsze właściwości psychometryczne. Zaprezentowano  także najważniejsze wnioski płynące z pilotażu oraz sugestie co do kierunków rozwoju na-rzędzia w przyszłości. Słowa kluczowe: zasoby, autonomia, przynależność, emocje, kwestionariusz

Introduction

What makes us flourish?

For many years psychology was dominated by a deficits-oriented paradigm (Selig-man & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). The issues of what exactly positive psychological functioning (understood as something more than simply a lack of disorders) is and

(2)

what factors make people function well are still very important questions for aca-demic psychology. Thanks to a considerably recent increase of research in this field, we can now formulate some well-researched answers to these questions.

Keyes (2002), who understands mental health as a continuum from mental disorders to flourishing, defines psychological well-being in terms of subjective satisfaction in three important spheres of functioning: psychological, social, and emotional.  Elements  that  should  be  taken  into  account  when  testing  psychologi-cal functioning consist of: self-acceptance, positive relations with others, personal growth, purpose in life, environmental mastery, and autonomy. Good social func-tioning means experiencing social coherence, social actualization, social integration, social acceptance, and social contribution. Finally, emotional well-being is under-stood as “a cluster of symptoms reflecting the presence or absence of positive feel-ings about life” (Keyes, 2002: 208).

Resilience/ego-resiliency is an important concept trying to answer the question

of what makes people function well in face of adversity (Luthar, Cicchetti & Becker, 2000; Charney, 2004; Heszen & Sęk, 2007). Usually it is understood in two ways:  as the process of resilience or as ego-resiliency. In the process of resilience there are some risk factors that influence a person (examples of such factors can be: low economic and social status, addictions and mental disorders in family, or disabilities). This influence is balanced by buffer factors like high esteem, a sense of self-efficacy (individual characteristics), family cohesion and warmth, close relations (family characteristics), a good-functioning school or having a mentor (external factors). As a result of this, the person functions well despite the adversity (Borucka  & Ostaszewski, 2008).

Ego-resiliency is understood not as the process of interacting both internal and  external factors, but as personal characteristics that allow a person to overcome adversity. To give some examples of such characteristics – a resilient person might interpret stressful events as challenges and think of them as a normal part of life, believe they can have impact on their life and the reality around them, and have stable and positive emotions etc. (Semmer, 2006; Ogińska-Bulik & Juczyński, 2008).

Finally, Deci & Ryan (2008) add an important, motivational element in the pursuit  of  flourishing  factors.  According  to  their  Self-Determination  Theory  (SDT), people have three basic, culturally universal needs: the need for autonomy,  competence, and relatedness. If these three basic needs are fulfilled, people develop the most functional type of motivation – internal, or intrinsic, motivation. This in turn influences their well-being and life-satisfaction (obviously in a positive manner).

These three concepts – the dimensions of flourishing described by Keyes, resilience/ego-resiliency,  and  the  Self-Determination  Theory  –  are  some  of  the  most important theories answering the questions presented at the beginning of this section: what does it mean to function well (apart from the absence of disorders), and what factors make us function well? The Growth Resources Model is another concept that may provide us with some important insights into these issues.

(3)

Growth Resources Model (GRM)

The Growth Resources Model was developed in order to grasp the key psychosocial resources helping us develop towards the positive end of the mental health continuum (Pasowicz, 2017). The model consists of three major components: positive autonomy, positive belonging, and positive emotionality.

Positive autonomy is defined as a set of key psychosocial resources allowing the indi-vidual to cope with reality in a constructive way and to achieve important goals. Posi-tive belonging is defined as a set of key psychosocial resources allowing the individual to build and sustain constructive and satisfactory relations with other people. Finally, positive emotionality is indicated by a dominance of positive emotions over negative emotions in our personal experience (Pasowicz, 2017).

The GRM is based on three most important theoretical and empirical sources: on  the  non-specific  development  factors  (Brzezińska,  2005;  Brzezińska,  Kaczan,  Piotrowski  &  Rycielski,  2008),  the  psychosocial  theory  of  development  (Erikson  & Erikson, 2013), and on research indicating the importance of positive emotions  (Fredrickson, 1998; 2001; 2003; 2013; Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005; Fredrickson,  Mancuso, Branigan & Tugade, 2000).

According to Brzezińska (2005; Brzezińska et al., 2008), in order to develop  well, people need two sets of factors: the first may be called autonomy factors (a sense of self-efficacy and personal control over what is happening around us, and a sense of autonomy in decision-making and in achieving goals), and the second – called belonging factors (a sense of security in relations with others, and a sense of good emotional contact with others, a bond, and a feeling of belonging to someone). According to Brzezińska (2005; Brzezińska et al., 2008), not only the level of these  factors is important for people’s functioning, but also the balance of these two sets of factors. The GRM follows this pattern of two interconnected ‘lungs’ of development factors, with positive autonomy and positive belonging as the two most important dimensions of the model.

In her concept of the non-specific development factors, Brzezińska2 defines the

psychosocial qualities following the theory of psychosocial development by Erikson  (Erikson & Erikson, 2013). That is why the GRM also incorporates the psychosocial  qualities described by Erikson (Erikson & Erikson, 2013), but its dimensions include  more characteristics than the non-specific development factors. How the qualities presented by Erikson (Erikson & Erikson, 2013) translate into the GRM is presented  in detail in Table 1.

Finally, the positive emotionality dimension is based on the growing literature and research on the adaptive importance of positive emotions, with Barbara Fredrickson as one of the leading researchers in this field (Fredrickson, 1998, 2001, 2003, 2013; Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005; Fredrickson et al., 2000). Positive  emotions found their place in the GRM also because we can hypothesize about their relation with positive autonomy (Fredrickson, 2013) and positive belonging (Gross,

(4)

1999; Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum & Ehlert, 2003; Ochsner & Gross, 2010)  in the form of positive feedback loops.

In order to empirically verify the structure and basic assumptions of the GRM, the Growth Resources Questionnaire was created and tested. The questionnaire may become an important tool for practitioners wanting to help their clients or patients develop and flourish, and also for scientist who are interested in exploring the issue of moving towards the positive end of the mental health continuum.

The questionnaire was tested on a group of 304 subjects, and its basic psy-chometric parameters, including factor analysis, are presented later in this paper. The questionnaire was also compared with the Polish version of the Mental Health Continuum – Short Form.

Growth Resources Questionnaire (GRQ)

Structure of the questionnaire

The Growth Resources Questionnaire consists of three major scales: the Positive Autonomy Scale, Positive Belonging Scale, and Positive Emotionality Scale.  Each of these scales consists of 30 items with a 5-point Likert scale for each item.

The Positive Autonomy Scale and the Positive Belonging Scale consist of simple  statements, and a participant declares to what degree each of the statements applies to them (‘definitely not’, ‘rather not’, ‘hard to say’, ‘rather yes’, and ‘definitely yes’). Scoring is very simple – ‘definitely not’ is 1 point, ‘rather not’ is 2 points, and so forth  up to 5 points for ‘definitely yes’. Some of the statements are formulated as negative,  and so they have a reversed scoring.

A base of items intended to measure psychosocial resources depicted by Erikson  (Erikson & Erikson, 2013) was created, and they were verified by competent judges  in terms of how well they represent the resources, and how easy it would be to understand them. Next, 60 items were chosen and are presented in Table 1.

As it might be seen in Table 1, there are 8 stages of psychosocial development  singled out by Erikson. In every stage, people face a developmental crises in which  they struggle with two opposite forces (i.e. basic trust vs. basic mistrust, or integrity vs. despair and disgust). If they overcome a crisis in a constructive way, people develop a virtue (i.e. hope and wisdom). Each virtue plays an important role in the  developmental stages that follow (Erikson & Erikson, 2013). 

In the Growth Resources Questionnaire each stage of development is rep-resented  by  6  or  7  items,  and  two  stages  (number  2  and  6)  are  reprep-resented  by  10 items. These stages provide more items in the GRQ, because they most clearly correspond with the non-specific development factors presented earlier in this pa-per (Brzezińska 2005; Brzezińska et al., 2008). 

As the reader might notice, the author decided to name the resources differently  than Erickson. The reason for this is threefold: 1) in order to depart from Erickson’s  virtue names that may seem a bit old-fashioned for modern psychologists; 2) to use terms that are presently used in academic psychology, and 3) to make the autonomy/belonging distinction more clear. Table 2 presents the resources divided into the tool’s major scales.

(5)

Table 1 . Psychosocial resou rce s of the GRM and ite ms in tended to me asure them Stage of deve -lopment Psychosocial Resources by Erikson Items

Category of resources in the GRM

Scale Number of items I Hop e, basic trus t vs . basic mis trus t I think tha t m y fu tur e will be g ood I c an’ t see fu tur e f or m yse lf Aft er dif ficult e xperie nce s alw ay s c ome s sol ace I oft en f eel despair I think tha t peop le c an be trus te d I think tha t peop le ar e g ood Hop e Hop e Hop e Hop e Positiv e a ttitude t ow ar ds other s Positiv e a ttitude t ow ar ds other s A* A A A B** B 6 II Will, aut on om y vs .

shame and dou

bt I c on sider m yself a discipline d per son

I usually bring things t

o an e nd I c an be de cisiv e I’m an indepe nden t per son I oft en le t othe rs de cide for me I think tha t m y fu tur e depends primarily on me Wha t is happe ning ar ou nd me depe nds on me I c an’ t accep t m yself I c on sider m yself a valuable pe rson

I’m not ashame

d of m y bod y Initia tiv e and per se ver ance Initia tiv e and per se ver ance Indepe

ndence and leader

ship

Indepe

ndence and leader

ship

Indepe

ndence and leader

ship A sense of pe rson al c on trol A sense of pe rson al c on trol Se lf-e steem Se lf-e steem Con

fidence and self

-e stee m in social c on te xt A A A A A A A A A B 10 III Purpose , initia tiv e vs . guilt My lif e has a me aning I t ak e dif fer en t action s t o achie ve goals import an t t o me I’m laz y I ha ve leade rship s skills In a gr ou p I oft en t ak e the rol e of a le ader I’m a con fide nt per son A sense of

meaning (or purpose

) Initia tiv e and per se ver ance Initia tiv e and per se ver ance Indepe

ndence and leader

ship

Indepe

ndence and leader

ship

Con

fidence and self

-e stee m in social c on te xt B A A A A B 6 IV Compe te nce , indus try vs . in fe riority I trus t m y abilities I c on sider m yself a compe te nt pe rson in the ar eas I’ m inv ol ve d in I c on sider m yself an in tellig en t per son I think I ha ve man y dif fe ren t t alen ts I c

an deal with une

xpect ed pr ob lems I f eel in fe rior t o other s I f eel tha t othe r peop le don ’t r espect me A sense of self -e ffi cacy , c ompe tence, and ability A sense of self -e ffi cacy , c ompe tence, and ability A sense of self -e ffi cacy , c ompe tence, and ability A sense of self -e ffi cacy , c ompe tence, and ability A sense of self -e ffi cacy , c ompe tence, and ability Con

fidence and self

-e stee m in social c on te xt Assertiv

eness and per

son al bor der s A A A A A B B 7

(6)

V Fide lity , ide ntity vs. ide ntity c on fu sion I’m f aith fu l t o m y partner I’m lo yal t o peop le close t o me I ha ve m y o wn pa th in lif e I kno w wha t I lik e and wha t not I ha ve m y o wn lif e s tyle I kno w wha t is mos t import an t t o me I c an’ t ans

wer the ques

tion of who I am Fide lity Fide lity W ell-de ve lop ed pe rson al ide ntity W ell-de ve lop ed pe rson al ide ntity W ell-de ve lop ed pe rson al ide ntity W ell-de ve lop ed pe rson al ide ntity W ell-de ve lop ed pe rson al ide ntity B B A A A A A 7 VI Lo ve , in

timacy vs. isolation

I think tha t I can lo ve another pe rson I ha ve close frie nds I f ee l e mbarr assed whe n someon e sho ws me a ffe ction I c an’ t s tand lon g in a close r ela tion ship with on e pe rson I f ee l lon ely I c an on ly c ou nt on m yse lf I f ee l tha t in a close r ela tion ship I lose m y individuality I’m a fraid tha t when I fall in lo ve , I will “lose m y head” I c an e xpr ess m y op inion withou t of fending other s Positiv e r ela tion s with other s Positiv e r ela tion s with other s Positiv e r ela tion s with other s Positiv e r ela tion s with other s Positiv e r ela tion s with other s Positiv e r ela tion s with other s A se nse of individua tion A se nse of individua tion Assertiv

eness and per

son al bor der s B B B B B B B B B 10 VII Car e, gener ativity vs. stagna tion Ther e ar e man y things tha t I car e abou t I lik e t aking c ar e of other pe op le Doi ng e ver ything on ly f or m yself w ou ld be meaningless It’ s import an t t o me to lea ve some thing be hind in this world I have dif fer en t r ol es in m y lif e I s till ha ve ne w plans and dr eams In e ve ry situa tion I w an t t o c ompe te with other s I ne ve r giv e up t o other s A se nse of positiv e c on tribution to socie ty A se nse of positiv e c on tribution to socie ty A se nse of positiv e c on tribution to socie ty A se nse of positiv e c on tribution to socie ty A se nse of positiv e c on tribution to socie ty Initia tiv e and per se ver ance Ability t o c oop er at e with other s Ability t o c oop er at e with other s B B B B B A B B 7 VIII Wisdom, integrity v s. de

spair and disgus

t I ha ve a big kno wledg e When I look back on m y lif e, I’ m pr ou d of m yse lf When I look back on m y lif e, I fee l gr at efu l t o othe rs I oft en criticise othe rs You

can learn some

thing import an t fr om e ver ybod y Pe op le g et on m y ner ve s The w orld mak es no se nse A se nse of self -e ffi cacy , c ompe tence , and ability Se lf-e steem Positiv e a ttitude t ow ar ds other s Positiv e a ttitude t ow ar ds other s Positiv e a ttitude t ow ar ds other s Positiv e a ttitude t ow ar ds other s A se nse of

meaning (or purpose)

A A B B B B B

7

(7)

‘A’s and ‘B’s in the fifth column indicate if a particular item was classified as  part of the Positive Autonomy Scale or the Positive Belonging Scale. Items in the PAS  represent resources necessary to reach important life goals and they represent the abilities to govern ourselves, sometimes against our surroundings. Items in the PBS  represent resources necessary to build and nourish relationships with others and are more connected with being a part of a bigger, social whole. To what degree the decisions to classify the items to the PAS or to the PBS were accurate will be shown  by the factor analysis presented later in this paper.

Table 2. Resources divided into the positive autonomy & belonging scales

Positive Autonomy Scale Positive Belonging Scale

1. A sense of self-efficacy, competence, and ability 1. A sense of meaning (or purpose)

2. Hope 2. Fidelity

3. Initiative and perseverance 3. Positive attitude towards others

4. Independence and leadership 4. Positive relations with others

5. A sense of personal control 5. A sense of positive contribution to society

6. Self-esteem 6. Confidence and self-esteem in social context

7. Well-developed personal identity 7. A sense of individuation

8. Assertiveness and personal borders 9. Ability to cooperate with others

Since the balance of autonomy and belonging resources is important (Pasowicz,  2017), by combining the results of the positive autonomy and belonging scales, we can place each participant in one of the four major sets of results presented in Figure 1. Obviously, we can also score autonomy & belonging together

(8)

The  Positive  Emotionality  Scale  is  somehow  different.  It  also  consists  of  30  items, but they are not statements, but names of emotions – 15 positive and 15 nega- tive. A participant declares the frequency of experiencing each of the emotions with-in the last month: ‘very rarely’ (1 potive. A participant declares the frequency of experiencing each of the emotions with-int), ‘rarely’ (2 potive. A participant declares the frequency of experiencing each of the emotions with-ints), ‘hard to say’ (3 potive. A participant declares the frequency of experiencing each of the emotions with-ints), ‘often’ (4 points), and ‘very often’ (5 points). The list of emotions consists of the  basic emotions presented by Ekman (1992), the selected emotions from Plutchik’s  psycho-evolutionary theory (Plutchik, 1980; Jarymowicz & Imbir, 2010), and other  emotions added as the ones often experienced in our daily life and deemed psycho-logically important. The decision which emotions to include in the scale was made by competent judges. A full list of the emotions in the Positive Emotionality Scale is  presented in Table 3.

Table 3. List of emotions in the Positive Emotionality Scale

Positive emotions Negative emotions

1. Joy 1. Fear 2. Acceptance 2. Disgust 3. Hope 3. Spite 4. Love 4. Anger 5. Happiness 5. Sadness 6. Admiration 6. Disappointment 7. Pride 7. Despair 8. Curiosity 8. Contempt 9. Delight 9. Anxiety 10. Pleasure 10. Grief 11. Trust 11. Mistrust 12. Fulfilment 12. Hurt 13. Surprise 13. Submission 14. Anticipation 14. Remorse 15. Calmness 15. Surrender In the Positive Emotionality Scale we are able to calculate a series of indicators.  We can calculate the intensity of positive and negative emotions separately, the emotional balance (positive emotions minus negative), the positive versus negative emotions ratio, and also the general intensity of emotions experienced (positive plus negative emotions). Results on this scale may be placed in one of the four categories similar to those presented in the part about positive autonomy and belonging. These categories are as presented in Figure 2.

Similarly  to  the  way  autonomy  and  belonging  are  understood,  positive  and  negative emotions form two related, but to some extend separated dimensions. This view of positive and negative affects seems to be empirically supported (Watson, Wiese, Vaidya & Tellegen, 1999; Larsen, McGraw & Cacioppo, 2001; Reich, Zautra & Davis, 2003). It is optimal when a person experiences 1) a high level of positive emotions combined with a rather low level of negative emotions at the ratio of

(9)

approximately 3:1 (top left). According to the chart presented, other possible sets  of results are as follows: 2) high levels of both positive and negative emotions (top right); 3) a low level of positive and a high level of negative affect (bottom right) – the most destructive combination; and 4) low levels of both positive and negative emotions (bottom left).

Figure 2. Possible sets of results in the Positive Emotionality Scale

Basic psychometric properties of the questionnaire

In order to perform a pilot test of the questionnaire, it was tested on a group of  304  subjects  (49  men  and  255  women).  The  participants  were  students  with  a secondary education and young working adults with academic degrees (174 and 130 respectively). The age of the participants varied from 17 to 37, with a mean of 22.8. Data analysis was performed with STATISTICA 13 software.

Basic psychometric properties of the three major scales – the Positive Autonomy  Scale, the Positive Belonging Scale, and the Positive Emotionality Scale – are pre-sented in Table 4. In the Positive Emotionality Scale, correlations of particular items  and Cronbach’s Alphas were measured in relation to positive emotions and negative  emotions separately, since it would be pointless to calculate these parameters for positive and negative emotions together. The potential range for each item is 1 to 5.

Table 4. Basic psychometric properties of the GRQ scales

Positive Autonomy Scale (Cronbach’s Alfa = .91) M SD Skew Correlation with the scale

Cronbach’s Alpha if eliminated

1. I trust my abilities 3.73 0.89 -0.74 .64 .901

2. I can’t see future for myself 4.36 0.9 -1.49 .61 .902

3. I’m lazy 2.93 1.16 0.04 .42 .905

4. I have leaderships skills 3.34 1.11 -0.32 .48 .904

(10)

6. I can’t accept myself 3.7 1.22 -0.66 .56 .903

7. I can’t answer the question of who I am 3.8 1.13 -0.81 .53 .903

8. I consider myself a competent person in the areas

I’m involved in 3.82 0.87 -0.83 .58 .902

9. I think that my future will be good 4.07 0.76 -0.57 .57 .903

10. I usually bring things to an end 3.88 0.89 -0.84 .41 .905

11. In a group I often take the role of a leader 3.11 1.14 -0.09 .41 .906

12. I think that my future depends primarily on me 4.04 0.81 -0.81 .34 .906

13. I consider myself a valuable person 4.27 0.83 -1.16 .67 .901

14. I have my own path in life 4.04 0.87 -0.7 .56 .903

15. I can deal with unexpected problems 3.83 0.71 -0.41 .52 .904

16. I often feel despair 3.56 1.11 -0.52 .5 .904

17. I take different actions to achieve goals important

to me 4.16 0.8 -0.92 .53 .903

18. I can be decisive 4.05 0.87 -1.05 .39 .905

19. When I look back on my life, I’m proud of myself 3.45 1.05 -0.3 .51 .903

20. I know what is most important to me 4.04 0.93 -0.88 .47 .904

21. I think I have many different talents 3.8 0.97 -0.76 .52 .903

22. After difficult experiences always comes solace 3.76 0.95 -0.65 .42 .905

23. I consider myself a disciplined person 3.3 1.14 -0.29 .3 .908

24. I often let others decide for me 3.65 1.01 -0.59 .44 .905

25. I know what I like and what not 4.41 0.62 -0.73 .41 .905

26. I consider myself an intelligent person 4.24 0.69 -0.59 .52 .904

27. I still have new plans and dreams 4.05 0.92 -0.8 .33 .907

28. I’m an independent person 3.63 0.95 -0.37 .41 .905

29. I have my own life style 4.28 0.71 -0.75 .45 .905

30. I have a big knowledge 3.63 0.81 -0.35 .49 .904

Positive Belonging Scale (Cronbach’s Alfa = .86) M SD Skew Correlation with the scale

Cronbach’s Alpha if eliminated

1. The world makes no sense 4.22 1.05 -1.22 .57 .854

2. I’m faithful to my partner 4.44 0.89 -1.51 .41 .858

3. I think that people can be trusted 3.44 1 -0.49 .48 .856

4. I feel lonely 3.43 1.23 -0.35 .57 .853

5. I like taking care of other people 4.25 0.81 -1.08 .41 .858

6. I feel inferior to others 3.7 1.16 -0.76 .5 .855

7. I feel that in a close relationship I lose my

individuality 3.81 1.16 -0.63 .43 .857

8. I feel that other people don’t respect me 3.76 1.16 -0.64 .45 .857

(11)

10. My life has a meaning 4.1 1 -1.06 .66 .852

11. I’m loyal to people close to me 4.55 0.61 -1.21 .41 .859

12. People get on my nerves 2.92 1.14 0.03 .5 .855

13. I have close friends 4.42 0.79 -1.49 .44 .858

14. I have different roles in my life 4.25 0.75 -0.92 .35 .86

15. I’m a confident person 3.28 1.12 -0.41 .4 .858

16. I can express my opinion without offending others 4.18 0.73 -0.66 .29 .861

17. I never give up to others 3.82 0.92 -0.47 .24 .862

18. I think that people are good 3.66 0.92 -0.6 .46 .857

19. I can only count on myself 3.64 1.12 -0.51 .42 .858

20. There are many things that I care about 4.08 0.8 -1.01 .27 .861

21. I’m not ashamed of my body 3.17 0.27 -0.29 .34 .86

22. I’m afraid that when I fall in love, I will “lose my

head” 3.75 0.38 -0.84 .34 .861

23. When I look back on my life, I feel grateful to

others 3.89 1.02 -0.93 .38 .859

24. I can’t stand long in a close relationship with one

person 4.04 1.12 -0.97 .4 .858

25. Doing everything only for myself would be

meaningless 2.92 1.52 0.08 .07 .871

26. I often criticise others 3.21 1.09 -0.05 .4 .858

27. I think that I can love another person 4.46 0.82 -1.72 .42 .858

28. It’s important to me to leave something behind

in this world 4.18 1.03 -1.35 .21 .863

29. You can learn something important from

everybody 4.29 0.83 -1.29 .31 .861

30. I feel embarrassed when someone shows me

affection 3.63 1.18 -0.52 .51 .855

Positive emotions (Cronbach’s Alfa = .84) M SD Skew Correlation with the scale Cronbach’s Alpha if eliminated 1. Joy 3.97 0.91 -0.71 .65 .825 2. Acceptance 3.8 0.97 -0.62 .62 .826 3. Hope 3.79 0.99 -0.75 .35 .84 4. Love 3.89 0.13 -0.81 .48 .833 5. Happiness 3.84 0.02 -0.62 .73 .819 6. Admiration 3.34 0.16 -0.33 .49 .832 7. Pride 3.11 0.1 -0.22 .5 .832 8. Curiosity 4.07 0.98 -0.94 .45 .835 9. Delight 2.96 1.21 -0.06 .58 .826 10. Pleasure 3.92 0.89 -0.86 .65 .825 11. Trust 3.78 1.05 -0.15 .51 .831

(12)

12. Fulfilment 3 1.22 0.29 .61 .825

13. Surprise 2.75 1.03 0.11 .09 .854

14. Anticipation 3.67 1.08 -0.86 .07 .856

15. Calmness 3.31 1.07 -0.29 .37 .84

Negative emotions (Cronbach’s Alfa = .9) M SD Skew Correlation with the scale Cronbach’s Alpha if eliminated 1. Fear 2.88 1.17 0.11 .55 .895 2. Disgust 1.82 1 1.22 .47 .897 3. Spite 3.26 1.21 -0.28 .56 .894 4. Anger 2.86 1.19 0.21 .57 .894 5. Sadness 3.26 1.14 -0.11 .64 .891 6. Disappointment 2.58 1.19 0.34 .66 .89 7. Despair 1.9 1.19 1.18 .73 .888 8. Contempt 1.76 1.07 1.42 .47 .897 9. Anxiety 2.96 1.28 -0.06 .62 .892 10. Grief 2.43 1.23 0.42 .67 .89 11. Mistrust 2.42 1.33 0.75 .52 .896 12. Hurt 2.23 1.26 0.69 .62 .892 13. Submission 1.72 1.07 1.31 .56 .894 14. Remorse 2.48 1.23 0.44 .46 .898 15. Surrender 2.02 1.18 0.84 .58 .893

The Positive Autonomy Scale. When we test a new psychometric tool, one of

the most important parameters is Cronbach’s Alpha, which measures the reliability  of each scale. This parameter for the Positive Autonomy Scale is very good – it can  be rounded to .91. Items that correlate with the scale strongest (at the level of approx. .6) are items no 1, 2, 8, 9, 13, and they are dedicated to measuring a sense of  competency, self-esteem, and hope.

In this scale no items could be eliminated and this way make the Cronbach’s Alpha  even  higher.  Items  that  correlate  with  the  entire  scale  at  the  lowest  level  (approx. .3) are question numbers 12, 18, 23, and 27. They might be replaced or improved in the future.

The Positive Belonging Scale. Although this scale has a lower Alpha, it is also 

at  a  very  good  level,  rounded  to  .86.  Questions  that  correlate  strongest  are  item  numbers 1, 4, 6, 10, 12, and 18 (correlations at the level of approx. .5 to .6), and they  are dedicated to measuring a sense of meaning/purpose, positive relations with others, self-esteem in the social context, and positive attitude towards other people.

There are 3 items that could have been eliminated or changed if we wanted to  improve  Cronbach’s  Alpha  for  this  scale.  These  are  question  numbers  9,  25,  and 28. The item number 25 correlates with the scale at the level of .07, which is 

(13)

unsatisfactory, and suggests changing this item or replacing it with a better one. Question number 16, 17, and 20 also correlate at  a rather low level, but if reduced,  they would not improve the Alpha.

The Positive Emotionality Scale. Finally, Cronbach’s Alphas for the sub-scales 

measuring emotions are .84 rounded for positive emotions and .9 rounded for negative emotions. The fact that Cronbach’s Alpha for these scales are so good is  a positive surprise, because various emotions, even if they have the same valence (positive/negative), may represent subjective experiences of very different qualities.

The emotions of joy, acceptance, happiness, pleasure, and fulfilment correlate strongest  with  the  entire  scale  of  positive  affect  (from  .62  up  to  .73).  This  may  suggest that these emotions are the most representative for positive emotions.

Two  emotions  could  be  reduced  in  order  to  improve  Cronbach’s  Alpha  for  positive emotions – surprise and anticipation. They correlate at a very low level of .09 and .07 respectively, which might suggest that the participants did not qualify these emotions as positive, but rather as neutral. This seems to be logical, because surprise might be both a positive experience (a surprise birthday party) and a negative one (an unexpected medical diagnosis). The same applies to anticipation – weather or not it is a positive experience probably depends on the object we anticipate. If it is something negative, we could as well experience negative affect.

As for the negative emotions, a series of them have the highest correlation levels  and could be considered as the most representative for negative affect. These are: sadness, disappointment, despair, anxiety, grief, and hurt (correlations at the levels between .62 and .73). In this sub-scale there are no questions that would improve  Cronbach’s Alpha if eliminated.

Summing  up  the  basic  psychometric  parameters  of  the  Growth  Resources  Questionnaire, it is fair to say that the most common indicator of reliability – Cronbach’s Alpha – is at a high and satisfactory level in all of the main scales of the  questionnaire. The levels of correlations of particular items with the entire scales suggest that there are only few items that need revision or replacement.

Factor analysis of the questionnaire

In order to analyse the structure of the two most important questionnaire scales – the Positive Autonomy Scale and the Positive Belonging Scale – exploratory  factor analysis was performed. The results of Cattell’s scree test are presented in Figure 3.

In the diagram, we can see that factor number one and factor number two have the highest eigenvalues (13.32 and 4.04 respectively), and with factor number three there  begins  the  scree.  Several  factors  that  form  the  scree  still  have  eigenvalues  higher than one (from factor number three on: 2.68, 2.41, 2.1, 1.76, 1.59, 1.54, 1.4,  1.29, 1.25, 1.18, 1.10, 1.07, and 1.03). Despite the fact that a number of factors have  eigenvalues higher than 1 (the Kaiser-Guttman criterion), a two-factor analysis was chosen, because 1) factors number 1 and 2 have the highest levels of eigenvalue; 2) several factors starting with factor number 3 form the Cattell’s scree, which might suggest that they are redundant; 3) two-factor analysis gives the simplest

(14)

and clearest results; 4) it corresponds well with the theory, and 5) such a solution  makes most sense when we analyse the factor loadings of two-factors with varimax rotation3 analysis presented below.

Table 5. Factor loadings of two-factor analysis with varimax rotation

Positive Autonomy Scale Factor 1 Factor 2

1. I trust my abilities .647 .218

2. I can’t see future for myself .453 .531

3. I’m lazy .285 .307

4. I have leaderships skills .593 -.004

5. What is happening around me depends on me .431 .248

6. I can’t accept myself .504 .436

7. I can’t answer the question of who I am .398 .494

8. I consider myself a competent person in the areas I’m involved in .625 .147

9. I think that my future will be good .458 .5

10. I usually bring things to an end .294 .311

11. In a group I often take the role of a leader .554 -.089

3 Orthogonal rotation was chosen even though the theory assumes that the positive

au-tonomy and the positive belonging factors should be related, because at the level of explora-tory analysis the goal is to verify if any structure might be identified in the data analysed. Or-thogonal varimax rotation is the most sensitive rotation strategy with respect to identifying factors within a given data set.

(15)

12. I think that my future depends primarily on me .385 .105

13. I consider myself a valuable person .628 .395

14. I have my own path in life .556 .267

15. I can deal with unexpected problems .514 .212

16. I often feel despair .330 .527

17. I take different actions to achieve goals important to me .483 .21

18. I can be decisive .404 .09

19. When I look back on my life, I’m proud of myself .411 .402

20. I know what is most important to me .356 .389

21. I think I have many different talents .647 .055

22. After difficult experiences always comes solace .329 .339

23. I consider myself a disciplined person .165 .246

24. I often let others decide for me .541 -.02

25. I know what I like and what not .413 .176

26. I consider myself an intelligent person .637 .048

27. I still have new plans and dreams .354 .074

28. I’m an independent person .542 -.051

29. I have my own life style .516 .076

30. I have a big knowledge .639 -.064

Positive Belonging Scale Factor 1 Factor 2

1. The world makes no sense .265 .596

2. I’m faithful to my partner .085 .456

3. I think that people can be trusted -.041 .613

4. I feel lonely .364 .508

5. I like taking care of other people .051 .496

6. I feel inferior to others .637 .308

7. I feel that in a close relationship I lose my individuality .104 .458

8. I feel that other people don’t respect me .295 .404

9. In every situation I want to compete with others -.218 .417

10. My life has a meaning .42 .633

11. I’m loyal to people close to me .242 .390

12. People get on my nerves .049 .618

13. I have close friends .236 .413

14. I have different roles in my life .287 .307

15. I’m a confident person .714 .129

16. I can express my opinion without offending others .214 .255

17. I never give up to others -.293 .478

18. I think that people are good -.021 .592

(16)

20. There are many things that I care about .174 .304

21. I’m not ashamed of my body .353 .222

22. I’m afraid that when I fall in love, I will “lose my head” .148 .345

23. When I look back on my life, I feel grateful to others .074 .472

24. I can’t stand long in a close relationship with one person .072 .43

25. Doing everything only for myself would be meaningless .081 .051

26. I often criticise others .022 .486

27. I think that I can love another person .263 .405

28. It’s important to me to leave something behind in this world .312 .114

29. You can learn something important from everybody .13 .334

30. I feel embarrassed when someone shows me affection .24 .468

Factor eigenvalue 9.30 8.06

% of the variance explained 16 13

Factor loadings ≥ 0,3 are presented in bold-face type

As presented in Table 5, factor 1 gathers most of the Positive Autonomy Scale  items (27 out of 30), and factor 2 gathers most of the Positive Belonging Scale items  (25 out of 30). That is why we can identify factor 1 as the autonomy factor, and factor 2 as the belonging factor. As this is a pilot test and analysis of the first version of the  Growth Resources Questionnaire, it gives us some important information on how the two basic scales of the questionnaire might be developed.

The autonomy factor. When we analyse factor loadings for factor 1, we can

see that three items from the Positive Autonomy Scale have rather unsatisfactory  loadings. These are: 3 (I’m lazy), 10 (I usually bring things to an end), and 23 (I consi- der myself a disciplined person). Such results might suggest that these questions do  not correspond with the autonomy factor well enough, and they might be improved or eliminated in the future. Psychologically they represent perseverance (10) and discipline (3 and 23).

Several items from the Positive Belonging Scale were gathered by the autonomy  factor: 4 (I feel lonely), 6 (I feel inferior to others), 10 (My life has a meaning), 15  (I’m a confident person), 21 (I’m not ashamed of my body), and 28 (It’s important to me to leave something behind in this world). Other belonging items that also seem to have significant influence on the autonomy factor include: a sense of positive relations with others (4), a sense of meaning (10 and 28), and self-esteem in a social context (6, 15, and 21).

Theoretically, self-esteem was divided into two factors: one related more with personal self-esteem, and the one representing self-esteem in a social context, but factor analysis does not support such a division. Items measuring self-esteem in a social context (6, 15, and 21) correlate better with the autonomy factor than with  the belonging factor.

The belonging factor. 5 items from the Positive Belonging Scale have unsatis-

(17)

16 (I can express my opinion without offending others), 21 (I’m not ashamed of  my  body),  25  (Doing  everything  only  for  myself  would  be  meaningless),  and  28  (It’s important to me to leave something behind in this world). As it was suggested  before,  it  seems  that  items  number  15,  21,  and  28  correspond  better  with  the  autonomy factor.

Items  number  16  (I  can  express  my  opinion  without  offending  others)  and  25 (Doing everything only for myself would be meaningless) seem to be somehow  controversial to the participants, as they have rather low factor loadings with both autonomy and belonging factors, which suggest they should be revised.

A series of items from the Positive Autonomy Scale correlate well with both  autonomy and belonging factors. These are items number 2 (I can’t see a future for myself), 6 (I can’t accept myself), 7 (I can’t answer the question of who I am),  9 (I think that my future will be good), 13 (I consider myself a valuable person), 16 (I often feel despair), 19 (When I look back on my life, I’m proud of myself), 20  (I know what is most important to me), and 22 (After difficult experiences always  comes solace). These items are intended to measure hope (2, 9, 16, 22), self-esteem  (6, 13, 19), and a well-developed personal identity (7, 20), and the results suggest  that these psychosocial competencies are related to both autonomy and belonging. Hope is also a part of the positive emotions list, so it is possible to eliminate it from the positive autonomy and belonging scales.

The complex and inter-related theoretical structure of the two most important scales measuring positive autonomy and positive belonging produces some diffi- culties when we want to perform factor analysis of the GRQ, as quite a few factors have eigenvalues above 1. At the same time, the theoretical assumption that the tool  consists of two major factors – autonomy and belonging – seems to find its empirical support: two-factor analysis stays more or less consistent with this theory and provides meaningful information.

Factor analysis also provides us with valuable information on how the ques- tionnaire might be improved in the future. Since it seems that autonomy and belong-ing factors overlap one another to a certain degree, it seems justified to develop the tool in such a way that these two dimensions form more separate and independent components. It is especially important if we want to measure the balance of these two psychosocial elements.

The factor analysis presented also suggests that it is better to develop a two-factor based model instead of developing an entire profile with multiple two-factors, because in Cattell’s scree test, two factors have significantly higher eigenvalues that the other ones, and two-factor analysis seems to be most meaningful.

The GRQ and Corey Keyes’ MHC-SF

Since  the  Growth  Resources  Questionnaire  is  designed  to  measure  the  key  psychosocial resources helping us develop and flourish, it is worth comparing its results with a questionnaire measuring flourishing. That is why 119 participants who filled up the GRQ were also given the Mental Health Continuum – Short Form  (Polish  adaptation  by  Karaś,  Cieciuch  &  Keyes,  2014).  The  MHC-SF  measures  subjective satisfaction in three areas: psychological functioning, social functioning,

(18)

and emotional functioning (Karaś et al., 2014). Since not all of the variables present  normal distribution, Spearman’s rho correlations were measured. All of them are  statistically significant and can be seen in Table 6.

Table 6. Spearman’s rho correlations of the GRQ and MHC-SF

GRQ/MHC-SF Psychological well-being well-beingSocial well-beingEmotional General

Positive autonomy .62 .4 .59 .61 Positive belonging .59 .45 .58 .62 Autonomy + belonging .63 .45 .61 .64 Positive emotions .50 .4 .8 .61 Negative emotions -.36 -.27 -.48 -.41 Emotions balance* .52 .4 .75 .61 Emotions ratio .49 .37 .69 .59 General** .63 .45 .71 .67

* positive emotions – negative emotions; ** autonomy + belonging + emotions balance

All of the correlations presented above are of medium to high strength, and  so, as it was expected, these two diagnostic tools are indeed connected. There is a certain correspondence between the major scales of the MHC (psychological well-being, social well-being, and emotional well-being) and the major scales of the GRQ (positive autonomy, positive belonging, and positive emotionality) – this correspondence can be seen in the correlations above. The most important difference  between  the  GRQ  and  the  MHC-SF  is  that  while  the  latter  measures  subjective satisfaction in three dimensions of human functioning, the former tries to capture the psychosocial resources leading to such satisfaction.

It is not a surprise that the strongest correlations are between the scales dedicated to measuring emotions, as Keyes (2002) defines emotional well-being as symptoms of experiencing positive emotions in the subjective experience. What is surprising though, is that positive belonging correlates at a lower level with social well-being than with psychological well-being (.45 and .59). 

Correlation  of  the  general  results  at  the  level  of  .67,  and  the  fact  that  all  of  the scales of the GRQ correlate with elements measured by the MHC-SF support  the hypothesis that the Growth Resources Questionnaire captures psychosocial resources connected with flourishing. Other tests should be performed in the future to further investigate the external validity of the tool.

Internal correlations

Apart from correlating the GRQ with the MHC-SF, Spearman’s rho correlations were measured also for the GRQ itself, and they are presented in Table 7. All of the  correlations are statistically significant.

The balance of emotions correlates with the emotion ratio at the level close to 1, because these two parameters are very close to each other. The balance of emotions is measured by subtracting negative emotions from positive emotions, and if it is

(19)

below zero, it means that a person experiences more negative than positive affect. The emotions ratio is the relation of positive emotions to negative emotions.

Table 7. Spearman’s rho correlations of the GRQ

Positive

autonomy belongingPositive +belongingAutonomy emotionsPositive emotionsNegative Emotions balance Emotions ratio General

Positive autonomy 1 .7 .92 .6 -.49 .63 .61 .86 Positive belonging .7 1 .92 .57 -.61 .69 .68 .89 Autonomy +belonging .92 .92 1 .63 -.6 .72 .7 .95 Positive emotions .6 .57 .63 1 -.48 .82 .75 .76 Negative emotions -.49 -.61 -.6 -.48 1 -.88 -.93 -.76 Emotions balance .63 .69 .72 .82 -.88 1 .99 .9 Emotions ratio .61 .68 .7 .75 -.93 .99 1 .88 General .86 .89 .95 .76 -.76 .9 .88 1

It is interesting to see how positive autonomy and positive belonging impact the experiencing of emotions. It seems that both influence the level of positive emotions, and at the same time positive belonging seems to have a greater impact on experiencing less negative emotions.

Positive autonomy correlates with positive belonging at the level of .7, and these results supports the notion based on the factor analysis presented earlier that these two dimensions might overlap each other a bit too much, especially if we want to measure the balance of these two factors.

Pilot group results

Finally, the results of the pilot test group may be presented. The reader will find them in Table 8.

Table 8. Pilot group results on the GRQ scales

N M SD Actual range Skew

Positive autonomy 304 114.68 14.56 64–145 -0.62 Positive belonging 304 115.25 14.05 67–147 -0.42 Auto/belong balance 304 1 0.11 0.64–1.38 0.43 Positive emotions 304 53.28 8.89 27–74 -0.29 Negative emotions 304 36.57 11.49 15–71 0.6 Emotions balance 304 16.64 17.44 -31–53 -0.42 Emotions ratio 304 1.64:1 0.69 0.5:1–4.31:1 0.82 General 304 246.58 40.46 117–330 -0.36

(20)

The pilot group represents an almost perfect balance of positive autonomy and positive belonging – none of these factors are at a higher level than the other. It might suggest that the group developed these two sets of psychosocial resources in harmony. Skewness suggests that these two factors are at levels slightly above the  average of possible range.

What is very interesting is the emotion ratio. Its mean value at the level of 1.64:1 is well below the approximately 3:1 level that seems to support flourishing  (Fredrickson, 2013). The balance of emotions seems to be the Achilles’ heel of the  pilot group in terms of resources supporting flourishing.

Conclusions

The Growth Resources Model is a new concept that may provide scientists and practitioners with important insights into the issue of personal development and flourishing (Pasowicz, 2017). In order to develop the model, the first pilot version of the Growth Resources Questionnaire was constructed and tested on a group of 304 subjects.

The questionnaire consists of three major scales: the Positive Autonomy Scale,  the Positive Belonging Scale, and the Positive Emotionality Scale. Cronbach’s Alpha  measurements proved the reliability of the scales to be good (between .8 and .9). Very few of the questionnaire’s items need revision in order to improve their correlation with entire scales.

Also  the  external  validity  of  the  questionnaire  seems  to  be  satisfactory.  The  GRQ was compared with the Polish version of Keyes’ Mental Health Continuum – Short Form – a questionnaire dedicated to measuring flourishing. All of the scales  of the Growth Resources Questionnaire correlate with the scales of the MHC – SF, as  was expected.

Factor analysis of the questionnaire was also performed, and it provided somehow mixed results. On the one hand, Cattell’s scree test seems to confirm the theoretical structure of the questionnaire with its two most important factors. Two-factor analysis also provides us with the most meaningful and clearest results, with factor 1 and factor 2 gathering most of the positive autonomy items and positive belonging items respectively. On the other hand, several factors have eigenvalue above 1 in the Cattell’s scree test, and two-factor analysis explains approximately 30 percent of the variance, which is rather unsatisfactory.

The results of the factor analysis provide us with most important information when we think about improving the questionnaire and the model itself. It seems a good idea to modify the autonomy and belonging scales in such a way that these two factors are more separate, consistent, and independent, with a more clear-cut division. It may also be valuable to shorten the entire tool by eliminating items that proved to be of little value, and to revise some of the decisions made concerning weather an item should belong to positive autonomy or positive belonging.

As the Growth Resources Model is a new concept that requires development and  testing, it is the intention of the author to develop a revised version of the Growth Resources Questionnaire based on the results presented in this paper. The revised Polish version could be then validated on a representative group of participants.

(21)

References

Borucka, A., & Ostaszewski, K. (2008). Koncepcja resilience. Kluczowe pojęcia i wybrane  zagadnienia. Medycyna Wieku Rozwojowego, 12 (2), 587–597.

Brzezińska, A.I. (2005). Jak przebiega rozwój człowieka? In A.I. Brzezińska (Ed.), Portrety psychologiczne człowieka (pp. 21–39). Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psycholo-giczne. Brzezińska, A.I., Kaczan, R., Piotrowski, K., & Rycielski, P. (2008). Uwarunkowania aktyw- ności zawodowej osób z ograniczeniami sprawności: czynniki powodzenia i czynni-ki ryzyka. Nauka, 4, 77–100.

Charney,  D.S.  (2004).  Psychobiological  mechanisms  of  resilience  and  vulnerability:  implications for successful adaptation to extreme stress. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 195–216.

Deci,  E.L.,  &  Ryan,  R.  (2008).  Self-Determination  Theory:  A  Macrotheory  of  Human  Motivation, Development, and Health. Canadian Psychology, 49 (3), 182–185. DOI:  10.1037/a0012801.

Ekman, P. (1992). An Argument for Basic Emotions, Cognition and emotion, 6 (3/4), 169– 200.

Erikson, E.H. (1998). The Life Cycle Completed. New York: WW Norton & Company. Fredrickson, B.L. (1998). What Good Are Positive Emotions? Review of General Psychology,

2 (3), 300–319. DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300.

Fredrickson, B.L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56 (3), 218– 276.

Fredrickson, B.L. (2003). The Value of Positive Emotions. American Scientist, 91, 330–335. Fredrickson, B.L. (2013). Updated Thinking on Positivity Ratios. American Psychologist.

Advance online publication, 1–9. DOI: 10.1037/a0033584.

Fredrickson,  B.L.,  &  Branigan,  Ch.  (2005).  Positive  Emotions  Broaden  the  Scope  of  Attention and Thought-Action Repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19 (3), 313–332. Fredrickson,  B.L.,  Mancuso,  R.A.,  Branigan,  Ch.,  &  Tugade,  M.M.  (2000).  The  Undoing 

Effect of Positive Emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 24 (4), 237–258.

Gross, J.J. (1999). Emotion Regulation: Past, Present, Future. Cognition and Emotion, 13 (5), 551–573.

Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., & Ehlert, U. (2003). Social Support and  Oxytocin Interact to Suppress Cortisol and Subjective Responses to Psychosocial  Stress. Society of Biological Psychiatry. DOI: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00465-7. Heszen, I., & Sęk, H. (2007). Psychologia zdrowia. Warszawa: PWN.

Jarymowicz, M., & Imbir, K. (2010). Próba taksonomii ludzkich emocji. Przegląd psycho-logiczny, 53 (4), 439–461.

Karaś, D., Cieciuch, J., & Keyes, C.L.M. (2014). The Polish adaptation of the Mental Health  Continuum-Short Form (MHC-SF). Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 104– 109.

Keyes, C.L.M. (2002). The Mental Health Continuum: From Languishing to Flourishing in Life. Journal of Health and Social Research, 43, 207–222.

Larsen,  J.T.,  McGraw,  A.P.,  &  Cacioppo.  J.T.  (2001).  Can  people  feel  happy  and  sad  at  the same time? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,  684–696.  DOI:  10.1037//0022-3514.81.4.684.

(22)

Luthar, S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: critical evaluation  and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71, 543–562.

Ogińska-Bulik, N., & Juczyński, Z. (2008). Skala pomiaru prężności. Nowiny Psychologicz-ne, 1 (3), 39–55.

Ochsner, K.N., & Gross, J.J. (2010). Thinking makes it so: A social cognitive neuroscience  approach to emotion regulation. In K. Vohs, R. Baumeister (Eds.). The Handbook of  Self-Regulation. New York: The Guilford Press.

Pasowicz, M. (2017). Introduction to the Growth Resources Model. Annales Universitatis Paedagogicae Cracoviensis. Studia Psychologica, 10, 23–38.

Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper and Row. Reich. J.W., Zautra, A.J., & Davis, M. (2003). Dimensions of Affect Relationships: Models 

and Their Integrative Implications. Review of General Psychology, 7 (1), 56–83. DOI:  10.1037//1089-2680.7.1.66.

Seligman, M.E.P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive Psychology. An Introduction.  American Psychologist, 55 (1), 5–14. DOI: 10.1037//0003-066X.55.1.5.

Semmer,  N.  (2006).  Personality,  stress  and  coping.  In  M.  Vollrath  (Ed.),  Handbook of Personality and Health (pp. 73–113). Chichester: Wiley.

Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The Two General Activation Systems  of  Affect:  Structural  Findings,  Evolutionary  Considerations  and  Psychobiological  Evidence.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 (5),  820–838.  DOI:  10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.820.

Cytaty

Powiązane dokumenty

In this work, the problem of determin- ing an upper bound for the cp-rank of completely posi- tive matrices is reduced to the problem of determining the maximal number of

The use of simple feed- forward in the adaptation loop (see Fig. 4) improves the robust stability of the control system. This approach is based on the concept of the “positive

We state new convergence results of solutions under assumptions concerning contingent derivative of the perturbed inclusion.. These results state that there exists at least one

Theorem 1 comes from investigations in harmonic analysis on groups (see [B1], [BSz]) and on perturbed cannonical commutation relations... Next we examine the deformed

The above considerations show that the knowledge of the structure of bijective linear maps on B(X) preserving operators of rank one (idempotents of rank one, nilpotents of rank

The concentrations of PM 10 and PM 2.5 , chemical composition of PM (16 PAHs and 7 heavy metals, respectively, gas chromatography and atomic absorption spectroscopy),

The diagram shows the cumulative frequency graph for the time t taken to perform a certain task by 2000 men.. (a) Use the diagram to estimate (i) the

The search for such dimensions was made empirically using indices which have been the basis of the World Economic Forum’s (WEF) Global Competitiveness Index (GCI) up to the