• Nie Znaleziono Wyników

The identity processes and the sense of identity: interrelations and significance to the capacity for closeness

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The identity processes and the sense of identity: interrelations and significance to the capacity for closeness"

Copied!
14
0
0

Pełen tekst

(1)

PL ISSN 0081-685X DOI: 10.2478/V1067-010-0142-3

Aleksandra Pilarska

*

Institute of Psychology Adam Mickiewicz University in Pozna

Anna Sucha ska

Institute of Psychology Adam Mickiewicz University in Pozna

THE IDENTITY PROCESSES AND THE SENSE OF IDENTITY:

INTERRELATIONS AND SIGNIFICANCE

TO THE CAPACITY FOR CLOSENESS

The article deals with the relations between the dimensions of identity postulated in the dual-cycle model of iden-tity formation by Luyckx and colleagues (2008) and the basic categories of the sense of ideniden-tity that – according to our own approach – determines the dominant ways of experiencing and understanding oneself (Pilarska, 2014; Pilarska & Sucha ska, 2013). In both of these theoretical proposals identity is viewed from a somewhat different perspective, although both are within the scope of so-called Eriksonian influences. In the article, we present the-oretical premises and empirical data concerning the relations between the two approaches to identity, and thereby we answer the question of what kind of experience of the self accompanies the intensification of exploration and commitment processes. Following up the thesis, commonly advanced in the literature, that identity is a precondition of the capacity for close relationships, we also address the possibility of explaining this capacity in the light of the two considered approaches to identity.

Keywords: phenomenological approach to identity, identity-related senses, exploration, commitment, identity status, attachment style

INTRODUCTION

The aim of the article is to answer the question of the type of the sense of identity that accompa-nies the intensification of exploration and com-mitment processes, as well as the question of the significance of these aspects of identity to the capacity for closeness in relationships. This is, therefore, an empirical attempt to integrate two ways of understanding identity: phenomeno-logical and developmental – an attempt to esta-blish their interrelations and their associations with one of the key criteria of their adaptiveness.

Both approaches are rooted in Erikson’s theory (2000, 2004). When defining identity, Erikson, on the one hand, stresses the subjective expe-rience (sense) of the continuity and consistency of the self. On the other hand, he devotes much space to the processes and stages of its deve-lopment – from diffuse, unclear representations of oneself (diffusion), through attempts at rigid identification with external patterns (totality), to integrated and at the same time flexible self-defi-nition (wholeness).

Since Erikson’s times, psychologists’ appro-ach to the issues of identity has evolved

(2)

conside-rably, with both of the above directions as points of departure for the emergence of two important research traditions: cognitive-phenomenological and developmental.

Identity: The Phenomenological and Developmental Approaches

From the cognitive-phenomenological per-spective, when starting out from the definitional question about identity: Who am I?, one is on the one hand pointing to the contents that are crucial for self-definition, and on the other – to senses that relate to his or her own self as a whole. This distinction, rooted in James’s (1892/2002) theory of the self, came to be regarded as the basis of the distinction between the subjective and objec-tive aspects of identity (Mandrosz-Wróblewska, 1988; Ole , 2008). The conceptualization of iden-tity in terms of a subsystem of self-knowledge has been an object of reflection and research in the cognitive tradition of personality psycholo-gy (Jarymowicz, 1989; Mandrosz-Wróblewska, 1988). From the phenomenological point of view, identity is treated as a special kind of personal experience of oneself and described in terms of a number of so-called identity-related senses (Batory, 2012; Brygo a, 2013; Pilarska, 2012b). The sense of identity is one of the key categories in psychodynamic theories (Kernberg, 1996) and self psychology (Vignoles, Regalia, Manzi, Gol-ledge, & Scabini, 2006). It finds its special place and clinical illustration in the field of psychopa-thology (Laing, 1999; Sokolik, 1996).

Conceptualizing the sense of identity as a per-son-specific, holistic, intuitive-reflective relation to oneself is one of the directions that we have been pursuing in our research for the last few years. It has a multidimensional character and encom-passes six basic categories: the sense of having inner contents (i.e., having and having access to one’s own thoughts, feelings, motives, attitudes, etc.), the sense of uniqueness (i.e., one’s own spe-cificity and otherness), the sense of separateness and boundaries (i.e., the ability to distinguish that which belongs to the self from that which does

not), the sense of coherence (i.e., consistency in terms of important inner content), the sense of continuity in time (i.e., the stability of one’s own self and being the same person despite the passage of time and despite the changes one undergoes), and the sense of self-worth (i.e., self-acceptance and a belief in one’s ability to accomplish personal goals). A detailed description of these senses and the basis for distinguishing them can be found in Pilarska’s publications (2012a, 2012b, 2014). Rese-arch results (Pilarska & Sucha ska, 2013, 2015; Sucha ska & Worach, 2013) clearly point to the adaptive character of strong identity-related senses and support the assumption that a mature (inte-grated) sense of identity includes the development and maintenance of all the senses listed above (cf. Sokolik, 1996).

The second of the two traditions mentioned stresses developmental and processual issues in the sociocultural context of identity formation. The significance of a person’s relations with the surrounding world in the course of life induced researchers to look into the dynamics and phases of identity formation. Their guiding idea turned out to be gradual exploration and tentative adop-tion of roles so as to try them out, before making a choice and identifying with the commitments concerning personal and social choices, tasks, and values. For years, a popular way of looking at identity in this tradition was Marcia’s (1966) understanding of identity as a dynamic organi-zation of drives, abilities, beliefs, and personal history as well as his first operationalization of the phases of its development, differentiated in terms of maturity level, called identity statuses.

The contemporary pace and scope of socio-cultural changes lead young people to postpo-ne taking on adult roles and tasks and involve a dynamics of identity that is greater than it was originally thought to be. This is reflected in empi-rical data, pointing to identity moratorium being extended beyond adolescence (Arnett, 2000; Brzezi ska, Kaczan, Piotrowski, & R kosiewicz, 2011). These observations induced a group of

(3)

& Beyers, 2006) to propose a modification of Marcia’s theory of identity statuses in the form of the dual-cycle model of identity formation. In this model, the originally defined exploration is understood as a broad search for alternati-ves in domains of importance to identity. It is called exploration in breadth – as distinct from exploration in depth, which refers to the evalu-ation of the commitments already made in terms of their compatibility with personal standards. The extension of this stage, connected with fears regarding one’s own future and with difficulties in making identity commitments, is described by the ruminative exploration dimension (Luyckx, Schwartz, Berzonsky et al., 2008). Unlike in Mar-cia’s model, according to Luyckx and colleagues the process of identity formation does not end when choices and decisions important to identity have been made (commitment making), although it may become weaker with age and as a result of engaging in professional or family roles (Brze-zi ska & Piotrowski, 2009; Luyckx, Schwartz, Berzonsky et al., 2008; Luyckx, Schwartz, Goos-sens, & Pollock, 2008). It continues in the evalu-ation of current commitments and in identifica-tion with those that one considers to be right and that one remains emotionally and behaviorally involved in (identification with commitment). If it proceeds correctly, it eventually leads to the achievement of an identity that is mature and at the same time open to changes. Marcia’s two--phase approach was thus replaced with a model of five interrelated stages proceeding differently in different individuals.

Identity and Close Relationships

The view that a formed identity is a precondi-tion of being ready to build close relaprecondi-tionships is commonly shared in the psychology of persona-lity, regardless of the adopted model of identity. That view refers to a comprehensive, wholeness identity (Erikson, 2000, 2004), formed thanks to the optimal resolution of developmental cri-ses; it also refers to a mature sense of personal

self/nonself representations and in the process of affective integration, experienced as a sense of separateness, consistency, continuity in time, or self-worth (Kernberg, 1996). A distinct aware-ness of one’s own values, beliefs, and of who one is as a person enables closeness that is free from the anxiety of losing one’s own individuality and boundaries – closeness that combines a sense of autonomy and interdependence. One of the goals of the presented study is to investigate whether and to what extent differences in conceptualizing the construct of identity are relevant in expla-ining the capacity for closeness.

The capacity for closeness with others is an issue approached from a variety of perspectives in psychology. It is sometimes described from the behavioral perspective, due to the balance of rewarding exchanges between partners, affec-tive, in which the main criterion is emotionality, and relational – taking into account many aspects of a relationship that continues in time (Prager, 1995). In the developmental perspective, the capacity for intimacy is treated as a result of reso-lving successive crises in identity formation (Erik-son, 2000, 2004), the phases of the separation and individuation process building the constancy of the self and object (Kernberg, 1996), or the attachment patterns formed in childhood (Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985).

Due to the quality of operationalization of the construct of capacity for closeness, the last of these models was used in the analysis of its associations with identity. In this model, adult attachment style is defined by the level of anxie-ty of being rejected and of the partner’s una-vailability or inattentiveness, as well as by the level of avoiding dependence on others, intimacy, and self-disclosure (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). The combination of anxiety and avoidance dimensions makes it possible to distinguish four attachment styles – secure, preoccupied, dismis-sing-avoidant, and fearful-avoidant – describing the habitual way of thinking, feeling, and beha-ving in relationships with close others (Fraley &

(4)

THE PLAN OF THE PRESENT STUDY

Research Questions and Hypotheses

The adopted ways of conceptualizing iden-tity and attachment presuppose their multidi-mensional character. It is therefore important to specify the relationships not only between ele-ments (dimensions, senses) in the whole group but also between specific configurations of these elements exhibited by participants. The former represents the variable-centered approach, and the latter – the person-centered approach. The specific research questions that we sought to answer were the following:

1. What relationships exist between identity--related senses and the dimensions of iden-tity development? Do people with different identity statuses differ in the ways in which they experience their own self in terms of the senses of having inner contents, uniqueness, separateness, coherence, continuity in time, and self-worth?

2. How are identity-related senses and identity development dimensions related to attach-ment dimensions? Do people with a secure and insecure attachment styles differ in terms of the intensity and configuration of identity development dimensions and identity-related senses?

The reflections presented in the introduc-tion and the available literature on the subject (e.g., Brzezi ska & Piotrowski, 2009; Brzezi ska & Piotrowski, 2010b; Luyckx, Schwartz, Berzon-sky et al., 2008) make it possible to formulate four hypotheses:

1a. The intensity of identity-related senses is signi-ficantly and positively related to commitment making and identification with commitment and negatively with ruminative exploration. 1b. People with identity statuses that assume

com-mitment making (i.e., foreclosure and achie-vement) have stronger identity-related senses

and a more integrated structure of these sen-ses, whereas people with statuses that involve problems with commitment making (i.e., dif-fused diffusion and ruminative moratorium) exhibit weaker identity-related senses with a less integrated structure.

2a. The levels of anxiety and avoidance are signi-ficantly and negatively related to the intensity of identity-related senses as well as to mitment making and identification with com-mitment, and positively related to ruminative exploration.

2b. Individuals with secure attachment are cha-racterized by stronger identity-related senses and a more integrated structure of these sen-ses as well as greater engagement in commit-ment making and greater identification with commitment than individuals exhibiting inse-cure attachment.

In order to test Hypotheses 1a and 2a, we per-formed an analysis of correlations (Pearson’s r), and in testing Hypotheses 1b and 2b we applied one-factor analysis of variance as well as the chi--square independence test. In order to extract groups differing in terms of identity status and the structure of the sense of identity, we perfor-med a two-step cluster analysis.

Sample and Procedure

The participants in the study1 were 228 people

(54.4% women) in their early adulthood (aged 18–35 years, mean age M = 24.16 years,

SD = 3.62), with different levels of education

and from various backgrounds. The study was carried out on an individual and group basis, with anonymity and the confidentiality of data ensured. All the participants completed measu-res of identity, and some of them (38% of the sample) completed a measure of attachment in adulthood. The participants were informed about the aim of the study, and their consent was the condition of participation.

(5)

Measures

Three measures were used in the study: one instrument of our own design and two Polish adaptations.

Sense of identity. We measured the sense of

identity using the Multidimensional

Question-naire of Identity (MQI; Pilarska, 2012a), whose

extended version consists of 43 items concerning six identity-related senses: the senses of having inner contents, uniqueness, separateness and boundaries, coherence, continuity in time, and self-worth. All the items are rated on a 4-point scale, from definitely yes / always to definitely not /

never, and the subscale score is the mean number

of points. Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for individual subscales reach the values from .62 to .86, the mean value being = .74 (e.g., Pilarska, 2014; Sucha ska & Worach, 2013). In the presented study, the values of alpha were between .66 and .88, with a mean of = .75.

Identity dimensions. Individuals’ position on

the dimensions of identity development postu-lated by Luyckx and colleagues (2008) – name-ly, exploration in breadth, exploration in depth, ruminative exploration, commitment making. and identification with commitment – was asses-sed using the Dimensions of Identity Development

Scale (DIDS; Brzezi ska & Piotrowski, 2010a).

The measure consists of 25 items, to which responses are given on a six-point scale from

strongly disagree to strongly agree. The subscale

score is the mean number of points. Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for particular sub-scales reach the values of .66 do .89, with a mean of = .79 (Brzezi ska & Piotrowski, 2009). In the presented study, the values of alpha were between .77 and .91, with a mean of = .84.

Attachment. To measure the dimensions of

attachment (i.e., anxiety and avoidance), we used the Experience in Close Relationships

Question-naire-Revised (ECR-R) by Fraley and colleagues

(2000; Wajs, 2012). Its two subscales together consist of 36 items, rated on a 7-point scale from

completely disagree to completely agree. The mean

subscale indicates his or her levels of anxiety and avoidance, and makes it possible to identify the attachment style characteristic for him or her. The reliability coefficients for both subscales exceed the value of = .80 (cf. Fraley, 2014).

ANALYSIS OF RESULTS

The Relationships Between Identity-Related Senses and the Dimensions of Identity Development (Hypothesis 1a)

In order to establish the associations between the dimensions of identity development and iden-tity-related senses, we performed an analysis of correlations. The obtained matrix (cf. Table 1) reveals a pattern of positive correlations of iden-tity-related senses with commitment making and identification with commitment as well as a pat-tern of negative correlations of identity-related senses with the three dimensions of exploration, particularly with ruminative exploration. These data are consistent with Hypothesis 1a. It is worth noting that the commitment dimensions are the most strongly related to the sense of self--worth, and exploration dimensions – to the sense of coherence. Relatively the weakest associations with identity development dimensions were found in the case of the sense of uniqueness.

Identity-Related Senses and Identity Statuses (Hypothesis 1b)

First we checked whether different configura-tions of identity development dimensions – that is, identity statuses – are accompanied by different intensity levels of identity-related senses. Cluster analysis yielded six identity statuses (cf. Figure 1), corresponding to the typology described by Brze-zi ska and Piotrowski (2009) as well as by the authors of the original version of the measure (Luyckx, Schwartz, Berzonsky et al., 2008).

Differences between the statuses in terms of all dimensions of identity development were significant: F(5, 222) = 92.03; p < .001; 2 = .67

(6)

p < .001; 2 = .65 for exploration in depth; F(5, 222) = 128.44; p < .001; 2 = .74 for

ruminative exploration; F(5, 222) = 100.16;

p < .001; 2 = .69 for commitment making; F(5, 222) = 97.63; p < .001; 2 = .69 for

identi-fication with commitment.

We observed significant differences between identity statuses in the intensity of all identity--related senses: the sense of having inner contents,

F(5, 222) = 13.04; p < .001; 2 = .23; the sense of

uniqueness, F(5, 222) = 3.64; p = .004; 2 = .08; the sense of separateness, F(5, 222) = 5.70; Table 1. The Matrix of Correlations Between Identity-Related Senses and Dimensions of Identity Development

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 1. SIC – 2. SU .18** 3. SS .33*** .19** 4. SC .76*** .14* .41*** 5. SCT .57*** .17** .25*** .60*** 6. SW .63*** .43*** .34*** .66*** .50*** 7. EB -.32*** -.10 -.24*** -.37*** -.26*** -27*** 8. ED -.21*** -.09 -.24*** -.28*** -.19** -.21*** .74*** 9. RE -.56*** -.16* -.35*** -.59*** -.38*** -.55*** .54*** .39*** 10. CM .48*** .13* .24*** .48*** .37*** .51*** -.23*** -.07 -.79*** 11. IC .38*** .16* .16* .38*** .32*** .51*** -.02 .14* -.60*** .79*** M (SD) (0.50)2.33 (0.50)1.88 (0.47)1.62 (0.50)2.07 (0.46)2.04 (0.48)2.06 (0.85)4.04 (0.84)3.87 (1.00)3.39 (1.10)3.93 (0.88)4.19

Note. SIC – sense of having inner contents, SU – sense of uniqueness, SS – sense of separateness, SC – sense of coherence, SCT

– sense of continuity in time, SW – sense of self-worth, EB – exploration in breadth, ED – exploration in depth, RE – ruminative exploration, CM – commitment making, IC – identification with commitment.

(7)

p < .001; 2 = .11; the sense of coherence, F(5, 222) = 17.08; p < .001; 2 = .28; the sense

of continuity in time, F(5, 222) = 7.35; p < .001; 2 = .14; and the sense of self-worth, F(5, 222) = 20.55; p < .001; 2 = .32. The results of post hoc tests (Tukey’s test or Dunnett’s T3 test) indi-cate that the directions of the observed differen-ces are consistent with Hypothesis 1b (cf. Table 2). Next, it was examined whether different iden-tity statuses were associated not only with differ-ent intensity but also with differdiffer-ent structures of the sense of identity. As a result of cluster analysis, we obtained three types of structures of the sense of identity (cf. Figure 2). Differ-ences between them in terms of all identity-relat-ed senses were significant: F(5, 225) = 163.86;

p < .001; 2 = .59 for the sense of having inner

contents; F(5, 225) = 25.02; p < .001; 2 = .18 for the sense of uniqueness; F(5, 225) = 34.35;

p < .001; 2 = .23 for the sense of

separate-ness; F(5, 225) = 182.48; p < .001; 2 = .62

for the sense of coherence; F(5, 225) = 79.02;

p < .001; 2 = .41 for the sense of continuity in

time; F(5, 225) = 103.42; p < .001; 2 = .48 for the sense of self-worth. Post hoc tests (Tukey’s test or Dunnett’s T3 test) revealed that nearly all clusters differ from one another in the intensity of all identity-related senses; only in the sense of separateness are significant differences not found between Clusters 2 and 3, and the sense of self-worth does not significantly differentiate between Clusters 1 and 2.

The individuals in Cluster 1 are characterized by the most mature and harmonious structure of the sense of identity, although what draws attention is the relatively weaker sense of uni-queness. Cluster 2 is composed of individuals with a less harmonious structure, distinguished by a strong sense of uniqueness and self-worth (possibly compensatory), which stand out aga-inst the remaining, weaker identity-related sen-ses. The people in Cluster 3 exhibit the highest Table 2. Comparison of Identity Statuses in Terms of Identity-Related Senses

DD (I) n = 34 CD (II) n = 10 RM (III) n = 28 U (IV) n = 58 F (V) n = 49 A (VI) n = 49 M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) EB 4.26 (0.48)a 2.82 (0.51)b 5.01 (0.48)c 3.68 (0.36)d 3.22 (0.62)b 4.80 (0.50)c ED 3.86 (0.42)a 2.28 (0.33)b 4.82 (0.50)c 3.53 (0.44)d 3.30 (0.60)d 4.65 (0.55)c RE 4.44 (0.59)a 3.56 (0.72)b 4.75 (0.53)a 3.31 (0.44)b 2.12 (0.47)c 3.21 (0.53)b CM 2.69 (0.67)a 2.68 (0.56)a 2.83 (0.80)a 3.86 (0.51)b 5.10 (0.59)c 4.59 (0.60)d IC 3.12 (0.51)a 2.74 (0.38)a 3.74 (0.39)b 4.03 (0.45)b 4.99 (0.61)c 4.87 (0.50)c SIC 1.98 (0.50)a 2.28 (0.42)abc 1.95 (0.51)a 2.38 (0.40)b 2.63 (0.42)c 2.43 (0.46)bc SU 1.66 (0.47)a 2.05 (0.55)ab 1.87 (0.57)ab 1.87 (0.39)ab 2.09 (0.47)b 1.83 (0.53)ab SS 1.52 (0.44)ab 1.57 (0.52)abc 1.35 (0.42)b 1.66 (0.42)ac 1.87 (0.43)c 1.56 (0.49)ab SC 1.69 (0.51)a 2.05 (0.35)abc 1.68 (0.53)a 2.12 (0.36)b 2.43 (0.32)c 2.12 (0.48)b SCT 1.72 (0.41)a 2.19 (0.52)bc 1.87 (0.49)ab 2.05 (0.36)bc 2.24 (0.43)c 2.10 (0.46)bc SW 1.61 (0.48)a 1.91 (0.30)ab 1.83 (0.41)ab 2.09 (0.31)b 2.46 (0.38)c 2.12 (0.46)b

Note. DD – diffused diffusion, CD – carefree diffusion, RM – ruminative moratorium, U – undifferentiated status, F – foreclosure,

A – achievement, SIC – sense of having inner contents, SU – sense of uniqueness, SS – sense of separateness, SC – sense of coherence, SCT – sense of continuity in time, SW – sense of self-worth, EB – exploration in breadth, ED – exploration in depth, RE – ruminative exploration, CM – commitment making, IC – identification with commitment. The different letters (a, b, c, d) in

(8)

degree of disorganization in the sense of identi-ty, with marked deficits in the senses of having inner contents, coherence, temporal continuity, and self-worth.

The relationship between identity status and structure of the sense of identity turned out to be significant: 2(10) = 77.93, p < .001, V = .41.

Individuals with identity foreclosure and achieve-ment dominate in the cluster with the most matu-re sense of identity, while diffused diffusion and ruminative moratorium are accompanied by the highest disorganization in the sense of identity. An unharmonized structure, based on a strong sense of uniqueness and self-worth, is found in people with an undifferentiated and carefree dif-fusion statuses (cf. Table 3). These results are consistent with the expectations formulated in Hypothesis 1b.

The Relationships of Attachment

Dimensions With Identity-Related Senses and Identity Development Dimensions (Hypothesis 2a)

The analysis of correlations of attachment dimensions with identity-related senses and identity development dimensions (cf. Table 4) revealed that an increase in anxiety is associa-ted with a decrease in the sense of coherence, having inner contents, separateness, and tem-poral continuity, while an increase in avoidan-ce is associated with a decrease in the sense of temporal continuity, having inner contents, and coherence. In the case of identity development dimensions, what turned out to be significant was only the negative correlations of anxie-ty with exploration in depth and ruminative exploration. Hypothesis 2a was thus confirmed Table 3. Analysis of Association Between Identity Status and the Structure of the Sense of Identity

Identity status DD (I) n = 34 CD (II) n = 10 RM (III) n = 28 U (IV) n = 58 F (V) n = 49 A (VI) n = 49 Sense of identity n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) Cluster 1 (n = 83) 7 (20.6) 2 (20.0) 3 (10.7) 18 (31.0) 33 (67.3) 20 (40.8) Cluster 2 (n = 74) 3 (8.8) 5 (50.0) 7 (25.0) 31 (53.4) 12 (24.5) 16 (32.7) Cluster 3 (n = 71) 24 (70.6) 3 (30.0) 18 (64.3) 9 (15.5) 4 (8.2) 13 (26.5)

Note. DD – diffused diffusion, CD – carefree diffusion, RM – ruminative moratorium, U – undifferentiated status, F – foreclosure,

(9)

partly, and more with regard to identity-rela-ted senses than to the dimensions of identity development.

The Relationships of Attachment Styles With Identity-Related Senses and Identity Development Dimensions (Hypothesis 2b)

In the next step, we tested whether people with different attachment styles differed in terms of identity-related senses and the dimen-sions of identity development. The group with a secure attachment style was composed of individuals with scores MPXFS than the median both on anxiety (Me = 2.47) and on avoidan-ce (Me = 2.15), and the group with an insecu-re attachment style was individuals with scores equal to or higher than the median on one or both dimensions (cf. Fraley, 2014). The analy-ses (Table 5) revealed that people with different attachment styles did not differ on the dimen-sions of identity development. What is observed is a significantly stronger sense of having inner contents, coherence, and temporal continuity in

Table 5. Comparison of Attachment Styles in Terms of Dimensions of Identity Development and Identity-Related Senses Secure (n = 30) Insecure (n = 56) Differences Variable M (SD) M (SD) U Z r EB 3.95 (0.93) 3.91 (0.85) 809.50 -0.28 .03 ED 3.80 (0.87) 3.77 (0.89) 823.00 -0.15 .02 RE 3.19 (0.85) 3.40 (1.04) 754.00 -0.78 .08 CM 4.11 (0.98) 3.79 (1.00) 689.00 -1.37 .15 IC 4.25 (0.95) 4.00 (0.79) 696.50 -1.30 .14 SIC 2.53 (0.26) 2.30 (0.44) 585.50* -2.35 .25 SU 1.79 (0.57) 1.88 (0.49) 764.00 -0.69 .07 SS 1.67 (0.41) 1.61 (0.43) 802.00 -0.35 .04 SC 2.28 (0.32) 2.08 (0.40) 594.00* -2.24 .24 SCT 2.32 (0.36) 2.00 (0.36) 461.50*** -3.45 .37 SW 2.07 (0.41) 2.04 (0.45) 817.00 -0.21 .02

Note. EB – exploration in breadth, ED – exploration in depth, RE – ruminative exploration, CM – commitment making,

IC – identification with commitment, SIC – sense of having inner contents, SU – sense of uniqueness, SS – sense of separateness, SC – sense of coherence, SCT – sense of continuity in time, SW – sense of self-worth.

Table 4. Correlations Between Attachment Dimensions and Identity Variables AnxD AvD SIC -.36*** -.29** SU .06 -.06 SS -.33** .01 SC -.37*** -.26* SCT -.32** -.34*** SW -.2 -.14 EB .15 .01 ED .26* -.07 RE .26* -.05 CM -.19 .01 IC -.09 -.02

Note. SIC – sense of having inner contents, SU – sense of

uniqueness, SS – sense of separateness, SC – sense of coherence, SCT – sense of continuity in time, SW – sense of self-worth, EB – exploration in breadth, ED – exploration in depth, RE – ruminative exploration, CM – commitment making, IC – identification with commitment, AnxD – anxiety dimension, AvD – avoidance dimension.

(10)

individuals with secure attachment than in those with insecure attachment.

We also compared groups with different attachment styles in terms of identity statuses and structures of the sense of identity. Analy-ses revealed a significant relationship between attachment style and the structure of the sense of identity, 2(2) = 6.12, p = .047, V = .27. More

than a half (51.6%) of the participants with a mature sense of identity were securely attached individuals, whereas the group with a deficient and disharmonious structure of the sense of iden-tity was dominated by individuals with insecu-re attachment (insecu-respectively, 76.5% and 71.4%). The relationship between attachment style and identity status turned out not to be significant (p > .05). Hypothesis 2b was thus confirmed partly and only with regard to the associations of attachment styles with the intensity and configu-ration of identity-related senses.

DISCUSSION

The common origins of the developmental and phenomenological approaches to identity – operationalized as its processual-behavioral (identity development dimensions) and expe-riential aspects (identity-related senses) – were used in the present study as a starting point for analyzing their interrelations. This enabled an empirical description of the subjective experien-ce of people engaged to different degrees in the processes of exploration and commitment.

The analyses of relationships between identi-ty development dimensions and identiidenti-ty-related senses showed, as expected, that commitment making and identification with commitment are accompanied by a strong sense of identity in all of its aspects. As predicted, the processes of exploration, defining the stage of experimenta-tion and uncertainty regarding one’s own role and choices, are accompanied by a weaker sense of having inner contents, coherence, and

continu-association with a decrease in the sense of self--worth. Such a clear relationship of this variable with exploration in breadth and in depth, and especially with ruminative exploration, may be revealing a kind of vicious circle mechanism in which low self-worth is conducive to exploratory tendencies, particularly to ruminative ones, bloc-king the transition to further stages of identity formation. This is consistent with the suggestions of Luyckx and colleagues (2008), who see the fixation on exploration as caused by the exces-sively high standards and strict evaluation of the quality of one’s own activity.

The experiential, “sense-based” descrip-tion of identity statuses that has been presen-ted supports the above observations and – as predicted – points to stronger identity-related senses in individuals who make commitments and identify with them (i.e., ones with identi-ty foreclosure and achievement statuses) than in those who focus on exploration, particular-ly on its anxiety-driven form. This is also cle-arly shown by the analyses of the relationship between identity status and the structure of the sense of identity – people with foreclosure and achievement identity dominate in the cluster cha-racterized by the most mature and harmonious structure of the sense of identity, while diffused diffusion and ruminative moratorium go together with the highest level of disorganization in the sense of identity. Thus, once again, the presence of permanent commitments turns out to be an indicator of the strength of the sense of identity. These results are consistent with other empiri-cal reports, in which identity statuses based on a high level of commitment – identity foreclo-sure and achievement – were similarly associa-ted with mental health characteristics (Brze-zi ska & Piotrowski, 2010b; Luyckx, Schwartz, Berzonsky et al., 2008). They also raise the issue, discussed in the literature (e.g., Pilarska, 2012b; Waterman, 2007), of the adaptiveness and func-tionality of the exploration dimension, which is decisive in differentiating identity foreclosure

(11)

At this point, it is worth noting the unexpec-ted advantage of people with identity foreclosu-re over individuals with identity achievement in terms of the senses of separateness, coherence, and self-worth. Can the slight weakening of these three identity-related senses in the form of identi-ty regarded as the most mature be interpreted as an increase in the openness and flexibility of the sense of identity? This would be consistent with Erikson’s idea of holistic identity (wholeness), but at the same time contrary to the results of pre-vious studies on the adaptiveness of the sense of identity, suggesting a linear positive relationship between the strength of identity-related senses and the indicators of well-being (e.g., Pilarska, 2014; Pilarska & Sucha ska, 2015). The first interpretation seems to be consistent with the intention of the authors of the dual-cycle model (Luyckx, Schwartz, Berzonsky et al., 2008). The model is based on the assumption that explora-tion and commitment are mutually interpenetra-ting processes, and – according to the adopted criteria of distinguishing identity statuses – indi-viduals with the identity achievement status continue exploration, whereas in those with the identity foreclosure status exploration occurs to a small degree. These assumptions are reflected in the construction of the method. Namely, the items of the Dimensions of Identity Development

Scale, including those making up the

explora-tion subscales – referring to ruminaexplora-tions about one’s own future, reflections concerning the commitments made, as well as doubts and fears connected with the shape of one’s future life (Brzezi ska & Piotrowski, 2010a) – are formu-lated in the present tense. If we agree that iden-tity foreclosure means absence or (temporary or permanent) abandonment of exploration and that mature identity achievement means further exploratory activity2, it becomes understandable

that the former is accompanied by an increase

in the strength of the senses of separateness, coherence, and self-worth and that these senses become slightly weaker subsequently. This, howe-ver, would call into question the current criterion of maturity with regard to the sense of identity. Resolving this issue, crucial to both approaches to identity, requires a separate study.

The relationships of attachment dimensions and style with identity-related senses are clear and generally consistent with predictions. Rela-tional anxiety and avoidance of closeness are associated with a weakening of identity-related senses, which manifests itself clearly when indi-viduals high in anxiety and avoidance are contra-sted with people with a more secure attachment style. Securely attached people free from anxiety and avoidance, are certain about who they are and maintain a strong sense of the unity and continuity of their self. It can be expected that they will seek and engage in mutual closeness without a sense of being in danger of confusion in the sphere of identity. There are also rela-tions, weaker than expected but theoretically important, between attachment dimensions and identity development dimensions. The positive correlations of exploration in-depth and rumi-native exploration with anxiety can explain both the instability of relationships that people enter into at this stage of their identity development as well as the extension of this stage. Assuming that – apart from work and worldview – a domain in which intensive exploration takes place is the sphere of intimate relationships (Arnett, 2000), and that the main aim of this exploration is to prepare to make a commitment to the romantic partner, it can be expected that the course of this process will be disturbed in conditions of anxie-ty in relations with others. The above observa-tions may be a kind of supplement for the results obtained by Brzezi ska and Piotrowski (2010b), where the level of (differently operationalized)

2 The classification of identity statuses proposed by Meeus’s team (1999) is worth mentioning here. These authors make a dis-tinction between achievement and achieving commitment. The latter status, combining strong commitment and strong exploratory

(12)

readiness for intimate relationships was associa-ted above all with the commitment dimension. The significance of identity development dimen-sions and identity statuses did not manifest itself in analyses taking attachment style into account, which may suggest that what plays a particularly important role in determining the capacity for closeness is the subjective experience of oneself. This is consistent with the psychodynamic appro-ach to the problem (Kernberg, 1996).

The multiplicity of models aimed at capturing the phenomenon of identity attests the impor-tance of the investigated issues and undoubtedly fosters constant expansion of knowledge in this area of personality. At the same time, it may cause confusion in a tangle of concepts and measures. The expression found in the litera-ture, “the psychological problems of identity” (Grzegorek, 2000, p. 53), reflects the difficulties encountered in working out a consistent appro-ach to the phenomenon of identity. This makes it even more important to investigate whether, despite definitional and operational differences, the proposed models in fact deal with the same problem – the experience of a person making the effort of self-definition. The analysis presented in this article is a manifestation of such investi-gations. The obtained results support the thesis that the two approaches to identity – phenome-nological and developmental – deal with different aspects of the identity phenomenon rather than with different psychological realities. This gives hope that it will be possible to achieve an integra-tion of the findings from both tradiintegra-tions.

REFERENCES

Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood. A theory of development from the late teens through the twen-ties. American Psychologist, 55(5), 469–480.

Batory, A. (2012). Wielowymiarowe i dynamiczne Ja podstaw to samo ci [The multifaceted and dynamic self as the basis of identity]. Roczniki Psychologiczne, 15(4), 137–159.

Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult romantic attach-ment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York: Guilford Press.

Brygo a, E. (2013). Podmiotowa i przedmiotowa per-spektywa to samo ci osobistej we wspó czesnym

wiecie [Subjective and objective perspectives of personal identity in a contemporary world]. Polskie Forum Psychologiczne, 18(3), 284–302.

Brzezi ska A. I., Kaczan R., Piotrowski K., & R ko-siewicz M. (2011). Odroczona doros o : fakt czy artefakt? [Postponed adulthood: Fact or artefact?]. Nauka, 4, 67–107.

Brzezi ska, A. I., & Piotrowski, K. (2010a). Polska adap-tacja Skali Wymiarów Rozwoju To samo ci (DIDS) [The Polish adaptation of the Dimensions of Identity Development Scale (DIDS)]. Polskie Forum Psycho-logiczne, 15(1), 66–84.

Brzezi ska, A. I., & Piotrowski, K. (2010b). Formowanie si to samo ci a poczucie doros o ci i gotowo do tworzenia bliskich zwi zków [The link between iden-tity formation, sense of being adult and readiness for intimate relationships]. Czasopismo Psychologiczne, 16(2), 265–274.

Brzezi ska, A. I., & Piotrowski, K. (2009). Diagnoza statusów to samo ci w okresie adolescencji, wy ania-j ceania-j si doros o ci i wczesneania-j doros o ci za pomoc Skali Wymiarów Rozwoju To samo ci (DIDS) [Dia-gnosis of identity statuses in adolescence, emerging adulthood, and early adulthood using the Dimen-sions of Identity Development Scale (DIDS)] Studia Psychologiczne, 47(3), 93-109.

Erikson, E. H. (2000). Dzieci stwo i spo ecze stwo [Childhood and society]. Pozna : Dom Wydawniczy Rebis.

Erikson, E. H. (2004). To samo a cykl ycia [Identity and the life cycle]. Pozna : Wydawnictwo Zysk i S-ka. Fraley, R. C. (2014). A brief overview of adult attachment

theory and research. Retrieved from:http://internal. psychology.illinois.edu/~rcfraley/attachment.htm. Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic

attachment: Theoretical developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered questions. Review of General Psychology, 4(2), 132–154.

Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item-response theory analysis of self-report

(13)

mea-sures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(2), 350–365.

Grzegorek T. (2000). To samo a poczucie to samo ci. Próba uporz dkowania problematyki [Identity vs. the sense of identity. An attempt to put the issues in order]. In A. Ga dowa (Ed.), To samo cz owieka [Human identity] (pp. 53–70). Krakow: Wydawnictwo UJ. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love

conceptu-alized as an attachment process. Journal of Personal-ity and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511–524.

James, W. (1892/2002). Psychologia. Kurs skrócony [Psy-chology: The briefer course]. Trans. Micha Zagrodz-ki. Warsaw, PL: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN. Jarymowicz, M. (1989). Próba konceptualizacji poj cia

“to samo ”: spostrzegana odr bno Ja-Inni jako atrybut w asnej to samo ci [An attempt to conceptu-alize the notion of “identity”: The perceived distinc-tiveness of me–others as an attribute of one’s own identity]. Przegl d Psychologiczny, 32(3), 655–671. Karbowa, M. (2012). Trafno metod pomiaru przywi

za-nia w doros o ci [The validity of methods measuring attachment in adulthood] (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Institute of Psychology, Adam Mickie-wicz University, Pozna .

Kernberg, O. F. (1996). A psychoanalytic theory of per-sonality disorders In J. F. Clarkin & M. F. Lenzen-weger (Eds.), Major theories of personality disorder (pp. 106–141). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Laing, R. D. (1999). Podzielone „ja” [The divided self].

Pozna : Dom Wydawniczy Rebis.

Luyckx, K., Goossens, L., Soenens, B., & Beyers, W. (2006). Unpacking commitment and exploration: Validation of an integrative model of adolescent iden-tity formation. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 361–378. Luyckx, K., Schwartz, S. J., Berzonsky, M. D.,

Soe-nens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Smits, I., & Goos-sens, L. (2008). Capturing ruminative exploration: Extending the four-dimensional model of identity formation in late adolescence. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(1), 58–82.

Luyckx, K., Schwartz, S. J., Goossens, L., & Pollock, S. (2008). Employment, sense of coherence, and iden-tity formation: Contextual and psychological pro-cesses on the pathway to sense of adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Research, 23(5), 566–591.

Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Secu-rity in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Monographs of the

Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2), 66–104.

Mandrosz-Wróblewska, J. (1988). To samo i niespój-no ja a poszukiwanie w asnej odr bniespój-no ci [Identity, self-inconsistency, and the search for uniqueness]. Wroc aw: Ossolineum.

Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego-identity status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3(5), 551–558.

Meeus, W., Iedema, J., Helsen, M., & Vollebergh, W. (1999). Patterns of adolescent identity development: Review of literature and longitudinal analysis. Devel-opmental Review, 19(4), 419–461.

Ole , P. K. (2008). O ró nych rodzajach to samo ci oraz ich sta o ci i zmianie [On various kinds of identity, their stability and change]. In P. K. Ole , & A. Batory (Eds.), To samo i jej przemiany a kultura [Iden-tity and its transformations in the context of culture] (pp. 41–84). Lublin: Wydawnictwo Katolickiego Uniwersytetu Lubelskiego.

Pilarska, A. (2014). Self-construal as a mediator betwe-en idbetwe-entity structure and subjective well-being. Cur-rent Psychology, 33(2), 130–154.

Pilarska, A. (2012a). Wielowymiarowy Kwestionari-usz To samo ci [The Multidimensional Question-naire of Identity]. In W. J. Paluchowski, A. Bujacz, P. Ha adzi ski, & L. Kaczmarek (Eds.), Nowoczesne metody badawcze w psychologii [Modern research methods in psychology] (pp. 167–188). Pozna : Wydawnictwo Naukowe WNS UAM.

Pilarska, A. (2012b). Ja i to samo a dobrostan psy-chiczny [Self, identity, and psychological well-being]. Pozna : Wydawnictwo Naukowe WNS UAM. Pilarska, A., & Sucha ska, A. (2015). Poczucie to samo ci

a przejawy zaburze osobowo ci: wyniki bada popu-lacji nieklinicznej [The sense of identity and symptoms of personality disorders: The results of a non-clinical population study]. Psychiatria Polska, 49(3), 599–613. Pilarska, A., & Sucha ska, A. (2013). Strukturalne

w a ciwo ci koncepcji siebie a poczucie to samo ci. Fakty i artefakty w pomiarze spójno ci i z o ono ci koncepcji siebie [The relationship between struc-tural aspects of self-concept and personal identity. Facts and artifacts in the measurement of self-concept complexity and coherence]. Studia Psycho-logiczne, 51(2), 29–42.

Prager, K. J. (1995). The psychology of intimacy. New York: Guilford Press.

(14)

Sokolik, M. (1996). Psychoanaliza i Ja [Psychoanalysis and the self]. Warsaw, PL: J. Santorski & Co. Sucha ska, A., & Worach, A. (2013). Z o ono

kon-cepcji siebie a poczucie to samo ci [Self-complexity and the sense of identity]. Roczniki Psychologiczne, 16(2), 199–233.

Wajs, M. (2012). Styl przywi zania a nasilenie potrzeb i obron narcystycznych u osób w okresie wczesnej doros o ci [Attachment style and the intensity of nar-cissistic needs and defenses in young adults]

(Unpu-blished master’s thesis). Institute of Psychology, Adam Mickiewicz University, Pozna .

Waterman, A. S. (2007). Doing well: The relationship of identity status to three conceptions of well-being. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 7(4), 289–307.

Vignoles, V. L., Regalia, C., Manzi, C., Golledge, J., & Scabini, E. (2006). Beyond self-esteem: Influence of multiple motives on identity construction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(2), 308–333.

Cytaty

Powiązane dokumenty

Pracownicy, którzy nie ukończyli 45 lat częściej deklarują zmiany po- staw wobec stopnia przywiązania do miejsca zamieszkania w porównywal- nych okresach (różnica o

W raz z rozwijającą się gospodarką system liberalnego kapitalizm u doprow adził do kryzysu rzem iosła oraz do olbrzymiej biedy w śród rzem ieślników ,

Founded in 1981 and based at the University of California, Santa Cruz, Shakespeare Santa Cruz is a professional resident theatre company which, like the Oregon Shakespeare

in perCeption of relations with parents To examine differences in relations with mother and father reported by participants classified into the various identity statuses in

Saavedra, podobnie jak Francisco Quevedo czy Baltasar Gracián polemizował z Erazmiańską wizją społeczności uczonych, República literaria jest wyrazem sceptycyzmu, niepokoju

Elity regionalne na Kresach Wschodnich dawnej Rzeczypospolitej przed I wojną światową — w czasie kształtowania się nowoczesnych ruchów narodowych i państw narodowych,

Potwierdza to druga mowa Piotra (Dz 3, 12—36). 26: ,,Dla was w pierwszym rzędzie wskrzesił Bóg Sługę swego i posłał Go, aby błogosławił każdemu z was w odwracaniu się