• Nie Znaleziono Wyników

Overview of individual identity metricsF-value (F, univariate)

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Overview of individual identity metricsF-value (F, univariate)"

Copied!
6
0
0

Pełen tekst

(1)

Overview of individual identity metrics

F-value (F, univariate)

The earliest and the most simple way of testing for individual differences in signals is to fit a one-way ANOVA with individual as the independent variable to explain variation in some signal attribute (e.g., Miller, 1978). F is calculated by dividing between group mean squares MS(B) by within group mean squares MS(W):

F=MS(B) MS(W )

(0)

The well recognized problem with F-values, however, is that F-values are systematically influenced by sampling; thus, they are not well suited for comparisons between studies. The two following metrics can overcome this shortcoming.

Potential of individual coding (PIC, univariate)

Potential of individual coding (PIC) is defined as a ratio of between-individual to within-individual coefficients of variation (e.g., Robisson, Aubin, & Bremond, 1993; Lengagne et al., 1997):

PIC=CVb CVw

(0)

Like the F-value, PIC is mainly used to compare the level of individual variation within each signal variable. For example, PIC was used to show that frequency variables are better suited to convey individuality than temporal variables in king penguins, Aptenodytes patagonicus (Robisson, 1992).

However, PIC has some shortcomings. It can only handle variables on a ratio scale (Robisson et al., 1993). Therefore, it cannot handle variables that can have negative and positive values like, for example, the slope of a frequency modulation. For this reason, PIC also cannot be used with uncorrelated variable components of original data obtained from Principal component analysis.

Furthermore, it seems that researchers have used two slightly different ways to calculate PIC. CVb in the formula is either calculated from all values (PICbetweentot) ( e.g., Owens & Freeberg, 2007; Charrier

(2)

et al., 2010; Reers, Jacot, & Forstmeier, 2011; Pettitt, Bourne, & Bee, 2013; Salmi, Hammerschmidt, &

Doran-Sheehy, 2014), or means for each individual are calculated first and CVb is then calculated from these means (PICbetweenmeans) (e.g., Lingle, Rendall, & Pellis, 2007; Lein, 2008; Reichert, 2013).

Beecher’s statistic (HS, univariate / multivariate)

HS is grounded in information theory (Beecher, 1989) and it is measured in bits. HS is based on F-value but unlike F-value, HS accounts for sample size:

HS=log2F+ n−1n (0)

It can be used for a single variable, or multiple variables can be subjected to Principal components analysis (PCA), HS calculated and summed over each principal component of an entire multivariate signal. A strength of HS is that it permits the estimation of a number of possible unique individual signatures within a population. Different variants of this equation have been used in literature. The source of confusion is the ’n’ in the formula. Total sample size (HSntot), number of groups (i.e., individuals) (HSngroups), and number of samples per group (HSnpergroup) could all be used as ‘n’ in this equation. Some studies explicitly state they used number of individuals as ‘n’ (e.g., Pollard, Blumstein,

& Griffin, 2010; Linhart & Šálek, 2017), but the properties of HS values in these studies sample independent as suggested in the original article by Beecher (1989). Yet another approach to calculate HS is to extract the variance component estimates and use the total (ơT) and the residual variance (ơW, associated with random factor) to calculate HS (HSvarcomp) (Beecher, 1989; Carter, Logsdon, Arnold, Menchaca, & Medellin, 2012):

HS=log2 σT σW

(0)

(3)

Information capacity (HM, multivariate)

It has been argued that HS is good for scalar variables and is not suitable in case of modulated signals (Searby & Jouventin, 2004). Therefore, HM was introduced to quantify the individuality of modulated signals. HM is derived from HS. Unlike HS, HM does not consider each measured trait (or principal component) but uses multivariate Euclidean distances between samples (distT = sum of distances of all samples from their centroid; distW = sum of distances of samples within individual to its centroid) to estimate F in the original formula:

HM=log2FM+n−1n (0)

FM=n−1

g−1(distTdistg∗distW W) (0)

where ‘n’ = number of observations and ‘g’ = number of groups (individuals). HM has been rarely used, and while HM values reflect the possibility of discrimination by humans and discriminant analysis, its superiority and relationship to HS was not directly shown.

Discrimination score (DS, multivariate)

To quantify individuality in acoustic signals, most researchers have used discriminant function analysis and calculated discrimination scores (e.g., Hafner et al., 1979). Typically, different features of a signal are measured. These variables (raw or after various transformations) are then subjected to

discriminant analysis and the total percentage of calls assigned to the correct individual is reported.

Several disadvantages of discrimination scores were previously reported including the undesirable effect that increasing the number of individuals in a sample systematically decreases discrimination.

By contrast, more calls per individual can improve discrimination scores. Due to these biases, researchers were reluctant to carry out quantitative comparisons among different studies (Insley et al., 2003). Despite reported biases, discrimination scores are widely used, probably because of the intuitive association with the task of individual recognition.

(4)

Mutual information (MI, multivariate)

Mutual information (MI) may represent an intermediate step between DS and HS and DS and HM. Similar to HS and HM, mutual information is grounded in information theory and expressed in bits.

However, it is calculated from a confusion matrix and has been suggested as a metric of goodness of classification that is independent of the number of calls and number of individuals in the sample (Mathevon et al., 2010).

MI=

i , j

log2(p (i)∙ p( j)p(i , j) ) (0)

where p(i) = probability of predicted individual; p(j) = probability of actual individual; p(i,j) = probability of match between predicted and actual individual.

References:

Beecher, M. D. (1989). Signaling systems for individual recognition - an information theory approach.

Animal Behaviour, 38, 248–261. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80087-9

Carter, G. G., Logsdon, R., Arnold, B. D., Menchaca, A., & Medellin, R. A. (2012). Adult vampire bats produce contact calls when isolated: Acoustic variation by species, population, colony, and individual. Plos One, 7. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0038791

Charrier, I., Aubin, T., & Mathevon, N. (2010). Mother-calf vocal communication in Atlantic walrus: a first field experimental study. Animal Cognition, 13, 471–482. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0298- 9

Hafner, G. W., Hamilton, C. L., Steiner, W. W., Thompson, T. J., & Winn, H. E. (1979). Signature

information in the song of the humpback whale. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66, 1–6. doi:10.1121/1.383072

Insley, S. J., Phillips, A., & Charrier, I. (2003). A review of social recognition in pinnipeds. Aquatic Mammals, 29, 181–201.

Lein, M. R. (2008). Song variation in Buff-breasted Flycatchers (Empidonax fulvifrons). Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 120, 256–267. doi:10.1676/07-067.1

(5)

Lengagne, T., Lauga, J., & Jouventin, P. (1997). A method of independent time and frequency

decomposition of bioacoustic signals: inter-individual recognition in four species of penguins.

Comptes Rendus De L Academie Des Sciences Serie Iii-Sciences De La Vie-Life Sciences, 320, 885–891. doi:10.1016/s0764-4469(97)80873-6

Lingle, S., Rendall, D., & Pellis, S. M. (2007). Altruism and recognition in the antipredator defence of deer: 1. Species and individual variation in fawn distress calls. Animal Behaviour, 73, 897–

905. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.11.003

Linhart, P., & Šálek, M. (2017). The assessment of biases in the acoustic discrimination of individuals.

PLOS ONE, 12(5), e0177206. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0177206

Mathevon, N., Koralek, A., Weldele, M., Glickman, S. E., & Theunissen, F. E. (2010). What the hyena’s laugh tells: Sex, age, dominance and individual signature in the giggling call of Crocuta crocuta. BMC Ecology, 10, 9–Article No.: 9. doi:10.1186/1472-6785-10-9

Miller, D. B. (1978). Species-typical and individually distinctive acoustic features of crow calls of red jungle fowl. Zeitschrift Fur Tierpsychologie-Journal of Comparative Ethology, 47, 182–193.

Owens, J. L., & Freeberg, T. M. (2007). Variation in chick-a-dee calls of tufted titmice, Baeolophus bicolor: Note type and individual distinctiveness. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 122, 1216–1226. doi:10.1121/1.2749459

Pettitt, B. A., Bourne, G. R., & Bee, M. A. (2013). Advertisement call variation in the Golden rocket frog (Anomaloglossus beebei): Evidence for individual distinctiveness. Ethology, 119(3), 244–

256. doi:10.1111/eth.12058

Pollard, K. A., Blumstein, D. T., & Griffin, S. C. (2010). Pre-screening acoustic and other natural signatures for use in noninvasive individual identification. Journal of Applied Ecology, 47(5), 1103–1109. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2664.2010.01851.x

Reers, H., Jacot, A., & Forstmeier, W. (2011). Do zebra finch parents fail to recognise their own offspring? Plos One, 6. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018466

(6)

Reichert, M. S. (2013). Patterns of variability are consistent across signal types in the treefrog Dendropsophus ebraccatus. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 109, 131–145.

doi:10.1111/bij.12028

Robisson, P. (1992). Roles of pitch and duration in the discrimination of the mate's call in the King penguin Aptenodytes patagonicus. Bioacoustics, 4, 25–36.

Robisson, P., Aubin, T., & Bremond, J. (1993). Individuality in the voice of the Emperor penguin Aptenodytes forsteri - adaptation to a noisy environment. Ethology, 94(4), 279–290.

Salmi, R., Hammerschmidt, K., & Doran-Sheehy, D. M. (2014). Individual distinctiveness in call types of wild western female gorillas. Plos One, 9(7), e101940. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0101940 Searby, A., & Jouventin, P. (2004). How to measure information carried by a modulated vocal

signature? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116, 3192–3198.

doi:10.1121/1.1775271

Cytaty

Powiązane dokumenty

[r]

Comparison of the audibility region for phase changes with the pro- perties of envelope detection by the ear has shown that envelope detection can only be the source of phase

The purpose of this paper is to compare the performance of discriminant analysis based methods for building credit scoring models (i.e. classical) against those based on the

Dwie uwagi o funkcjach typowo-rzeczywistych Две заметки о типично-вещественных

Salvatore Antonello Parente, reprezentującego Uniwersytet degli studi di Bari Aldo Moro, który przed- stawił wystąpienie dotyczące styku cyberprzestrzeni oraz prawa finanso- wego

We provided an overview of the metrics used to quantify individual identity in animal signals in order 351. to identify the best method for reporting individuality in

W tworzeniu i zaleca- niu układów odniesienia należy przestrzegać następujących zasad: w przypadku pomiarów, na które może mieć wpływ nadmierna zawartość

Tomasz Janowski is the Head of the Department of Applied Informatics in Management at the Faculty of Economics and Management, Gdańsk University of Technology, Poland;