• Nie Znaleziono Wyników

Regulatory foci and the big fiveWacławBąk

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Share "Regulatory foci and the big fiveWacławBąk"

Copied!
1
0
0

Pełen tekst

(1)

The 11th European Congress of Psychology, Oslo, Norway 7-10 july 2009

Regulatory foci and the big five

Wacław Bąk

1

Institute of Psychology, Chair of Personality Psychology The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Poland

I N T R O D U C T I O N

Regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 1997) distinguishes between two modes of self-regulation: (1) promotion regulatory focus - concerned with ideals, advancement, aspiration, growth, and accomplishment, (2) prevention regulatory focus - concerned with oughts, protection, safety, and responsibility (Higgins, Spiegel, 2004). Promotion focus serves nurturance needs and involves orientation to presence or absence of positive outcomes, whereas prevention focus serves security needs and involves orientation to presence or absence of negative outcomes. In achieving their goals, promotion focused people prefer to use eagerness means and they are risky biased, in contrast, prevention focused people prefer to use vigilance means and are conservative biased. According to theory, regulatory focus can be analyzed both as a situationally induced orientation and as a relatively stable, individual-difference variable. The latter is assumed to develop as an effect of the quality of interactions with caretakers in childhood (Keller, 2008). Even if the theory is right there is still the possibility that some innate, stable personality variables may account for individual mode of self-regulation (see: Manian, Strauman, Denney, 1998). Little is known however about personality correlates of regulatory foci.

H Y P O T H E S E S

The present study explores the relations between regulatory focus and classic individual difference variables, represented by FFM personality traits (Costa &

McCrae, 1992). It is hypothesized that

1. Promotion focus is related to and can be predicted by extraversion (E) and openness to experience (O).

2. Prevention focus is related to and can be predicted by neuroticism (N) and conscientiousness (C).

P A R T I C I P A N T S

The sample comprised 86 participants, of whom 52 (60%) were women, with an average age of 21,8 (SD=1,37).

M E A S U R E S

 NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) (Costa & McCrae) - Polish adaptation by Zawadzki, Strelau, Szczepaniak & Śliwińska (1998)

Regulatory foci were measured by 2 independent inventories:

1. Regulatory Focus Scale (RFS) (Fellner, Holler, Kirchler & Schabmann, 2007) - Polish adaptation by Bąk & Łaguna (in progress) with modification of scoring procedure: (1) promotion scale (items: 4, 6, 8) - α=0,68; (2) prevention scale (items: 2, 3, 7)- α=0,63

2. Regulatory Focus Questionnaire (RFQ) (Higgins et. al, 2001) - Polish adaptation by Drogosz with modification of scoring procedure (W. Bąk): (1) promotion

prom_RFS prev_RFS prom_RFQ prev_RFQ promotion_RFS -

prevention_RFS ,329** -

promotion_RFQ ,415*** ,253* -

prevention_RFQ -,164 ,216* ,041 -

Neuroticism -,424*** -,238* -,576*** ,024 Extraversion ,699*** ,076 ,414*** -,272*

Openness ,371*** -,057 ,232* -,262*

Agreeableness -,063 ,209 -,038 ,296**

Conscientiousness ,317** ,539*** ,538** ,236* C O R R E L A T I O N S among personality traits and regulatory foci

E O A C N

promotion

prevention

,37***

- ,36***

,26**

- ,30**

,26**

,23**

R2=0,50***

R2=0,19***

RFQ

E

O A C N

promotion

prevention

,13 - ,22**

,52***

,53**

R2=0,50***

R2=0,27***

RFS

,15

S U M M A R Y

 Regulatory foci are significantly related to dimensions of five factor model.

 Pattern of relations is close to what was postulated, though not all predictions are confirmed.

 Dispositional promotion focus is best predicted by extraversion and neuroticism, while the role of openness and conscientiousness is unclear and needs further verification.

 Dispositional prevention focus is best predicted by conscientiousness.

 The relations between extraversion, agreeableness and prevention focus are probably caused by specificity of RFQ prevention items - all of them refer to memories of social interactions with parents in childhood.

 Significant difference of pattern of results between RFS and RFQ raises the question about validity of these instruments. Do they measure the same constructs and which one is closer to Higgins’ theory?

R E F E R E N C E S

Fellner, B., Holler, M., Kirchler, E. & Schabmann, A. (2007). Regulatory Focus Scale (RFS): Development of a scale to record dispositional regulatory focus. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 66, 109-116.

Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280-1300.

Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson, L. C., Ayduk, O. N., Taylor, A. (2001). Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: promotion pride versus prevention pride. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 3-23.

Higgins, E. T., Spiegel, S. (2004). Promotion and prevention strategies for self-regulation. A motivated cognition perspective.

W: R. F. Baumeister, K. D. Vohs (red.), Handbook of self-regulation. Research, theory, and applications (s. 171-187). New York / London: The Guilford Press.

Keller, J. (2008). On the development of regulatory focus: The role of parenting style. European Journal of Psychology, 38, 354-364.

Manian, N., Strauman, T. J., Denney, N. (1998). Temperament, recalled parenting styles, and self-regulation: Testing the developmental postulates of self-discrepancy theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1321-1332.

Zawadzki, B., Strelau, J., Szczepanik, P., Śliwińska, M. (1998). Inwentarz osobowości NEO-FFI Costy i McCrae. Warszawa:

Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.

by Drogosz with modification of scoring procedure (W. Bąk): (1) promotion scale (items: 1, 7, 9, 10) - α=0,60; (2) prevention scale (items: 2, 4, 5, 6)- α=0,79

R E G R E S S I O N A N A L Y S E S

Conscientiousness ,317** ,539*** ,538** ,236*

1The study was part of a broader research project in which participated also: J. Adamiec, A. Chałubińska-Gonerko, S.

Ciastek, T. Korulczyk, K. Kubica, E. Pałka, N. Zagrodzka, M.

Załęska

wabak@kul.lublin.pl

*** p<0,001; ** p<0,01; * p<0,05

*** p<0,001; ** p<0,01; * p<0,05

Cytaty

Powiązane dokumenty

OUT1 OUT2 OUT3 OUT4 OUT5 OUT6 C2 OUT7 OUT8 OUT9 OUT10 OUT11 OUT12 ALARM-C ALARM-NC ALARM-NO TERMISTOR NTC RS485. REGULATOR LRM001 L1 L2

Porównanie miar rzetelności obu kwestionariuszy wskazuje, że rzetelność skali promocyjnej RFS, szaco- wana za pomocą α Cronbacha, jest istotnie statystycz- nie wyższa w porównaniu

Zastosowanie pamięci programowej typu Flash EPROM stwarza możliwość fabrycznego wyposażania regulatorów w bibliotekę aplikacji standardowych, adaptacji tych aplikacji do danego

• W klimatyzacji i ciepłownictwie znalazły szerokie zastosowanie jako urządzenia zabezpieczające przed niedopuszczalnym spadkiem lub wzrostem temperatury (termostaty) oraz

• W skład tego sterownika wchodzi swobodnie programowalny sterownik kompaktowy o określonej liczbie wejść/wyjść oraz dowolnie konfigurowana dodatkowa liczba modułów

• W przypadku regulatorów PID są to: zakres proporcjonalności Xp, czas zdwojenia (całkowania) Ti oraz czas wyprzedzenia (różniczkowania) Td. • Opracowano wiele metod doboru

• W skład tego sterownika wchodzi swobodnie programowalny sterownik kompaktowy o określonej liczbie wejść/wyjść oraz dowolnie konfigurowana dodatkowa liczba modułów

• W skład tego sterownika wchodzi swobodnie programowalny sterownik kompaktowy o określonej liczbie wejść/wyjść oraz dowolnie konfigurowana dodatkowa liczba modułów